Meng Zheng, Lizhi Zhou, Niannian Zhao, Wenbin Xu. 2015: Effects of variation in food resources on foraging habitat use by wintering Hooded Cranes (Grus monacha). Avian Research, 6(1): 11. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-015-0020-3
Citation: Meng Zheng, Lizhi Zhou, Niannian Zhao, Wenbin Xu. 2015: Effects of variation in food resources on foraging habitat use by wintering Hooded Cranes (Grus monacha). Avian Research, 6(1): 11. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-015-0020-3

Effects of variation in food resources on foraging habitat use by wintering Hooded Cranes (Grus monacha)

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Lizhi Zhou, zhoulz@ahu.edu.cn

  • Received Date: 30 Sep 2014
  • Accepted Date: 15 Apr 2015
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 30 Jun 2015
  • Background 

    The ideal habitat use of waterbirds can be considered to be fixed, but current habitat use depends on environmental conditions, especially those of food characteristics, considered crucial to their use of habitats. Understanding how waterbirds respond to variation in food availability at degraded wetland sites and change their habitat use patterns over spatial and temporal scales should direct future conservation planning. The Objectives of this study were to identify these spatial-temporal foraging habitat use patterns of Hooded Cranes (Grus monacha) and their relationship with food characteristics in the severely degraded wetlands of the Shengjin and Caizi lakes along with the Yangtze River floodplain.

    Methods 

    We investigated the changes in food characteristics, relative abundance and density of Hooded Cranes in various habitat types across three winter periods from November 2012 to April 2013. We examined the effect of these winter periods and habitat types on the pattern of use by the cranes and explored the relationship between these patterns and food characteristics using linear regression.

    Results 

    The food characteristics and habitat use clearly changed over spatial-temporal scales. In the early and mid-winter periods, the most abundant, accessible and frequented food resources were found in paddy fields, while in the late period the more abundant food were available in meadows, which then replaced the paddy fields. There were fewer effects of winter periods, habitat types and their interactions on habitat use patterns except for the effect of habitat types on the relative abundance, determined as a function of food abundance, but independent of food depth and sediment permeability.

    Conclusions 

    In response to the degradation and loss of lake wetlands, the cranes shifted their habitat use patterns by making tradeoffs between food abundance and accessibility over spatial-temporal scales that facilitated their survival in the mosaic of these lake wetlands.

  • Avian migration is a spectacular phenomenon that has fascinated humans for centuries. However, many aspects of avian migration biology and particularly of navigation still remain unresolved (Alerstam, 2006). Billions of birds annually move between their breeding and non-breeding areas and many of them show remarkable natal, breeding and wintering site fidelity (Sokolov, 1997; Newton, 2008). To do this, they obviously have to be able to select and maintain flight directions (i.e. to perform migratory orientation) and to find the goal of their movements without any direct sensory contact (i.e. to navigate).

    Migrating birds are able to use an array of compass cues for orientation and complex interactions between different cues apparently exist. In recent years, significant advances have been made in understanding compass mechanisms that migrating birds use to determine and maintain direction of their movements (for a review see Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 2009). Particularly, considerable progress has been achieved in understanding the physiological basis of magnetoreception. The birds seem to possess two independent magnetoreception systems, one in their upper beak (Fleissner et al., 2003, 2007; Falkenberg et al., 2010) and one in the retina of their eye (Mouritsen et al., 2004; Mouritsen and Ritz, 2005; Ritz et al., 2010). Only the system localized in the retina is used as a compass, whereas the upper beak magnetoreceptor is not necessary for orientation (Zapka et al., 2009) and might be responsible for the magnetic map sense. The compass system of migrating birds is apparently redundant, i.e. they use several different compass cues, including celestial, magnetic and possibly also olfactory ones and somehow integrate information that they obtain from these cues. How exactly migrants integrate these cues, how and when they calibrate them and which compass system has priority over others is a subject of debate.

    It has been suggested that the relative importance of celestial and magnetic cues varies between the pre-migratory and migratory periods (Wiltschko et al., 1998a, 1998b; Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1999). During the pre-migratory period, celestial cues provide birds with their primary calibration reference; the magnetic compass is recalibrated when birds are exposed to a rotating magnetic field in the presence of natural celestial cues (Prinz and Wiltschko, 1992; Weindler and Liepa, 1999). Conversely, in most experiments during migration, birds exposed to a rotating magnetic field showed a corresponding shift in the direction of migratory orientation, indicating a precedence of magnetic cues (Wiltschko et al., 1998b, 1999; Sandberg et al., 2000). When tested for after-effects under natural celestial cues in the absence of magnetic cues (vertical magnetic field), the birds maintained the shifted direction, suggesting recalibration of their celestial compass relative to the magnetic field (Bingman and Wiltschko, 1988; Wiltschko et al., 1998b, 1999, 2001; Sandberg et al., 1991, 2000). In the study by Åkesson et al. (2002), juvenile White-crowned Sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys gambelii) showed orientation following the rotation of the magnetic field (90° counterclockwise) even when tested in the natural magnetic field (not in the vertical magnetic field) after exposure to cue-conflict, which did not occur at sunset in that study.

    The only study that contradicted this apparently coherent pattern was that by Able and Able (1995), who showed that both first-autumn and adult Savannah Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis) adjusted their migratory direction as if they were calibrating their magnetic compass relative to celestial cues, not vice versa, as in other studies.

    However, more recently Muheim et al. (2006a) reviewed a large body of evidence and detected another pattern. These authors found that responses to cue conflict, either before the onset of migration or during migration, were correlated with access to celestial cues. Birds exposed to cue conflict during sunset used sunset information as the primary cue if they had an unobstructed view of the entire sky, including its part near the horizon. Conversely, if cue conflict experiments were conducted at times of day that did not include sunset or the sky could not be seen near the horizon, the birds would use the geomagnetic field as their primary compass cue. It should be noted that the importance of seeing the sky near the horizon for the correct use of celestial cues had been suggested by Sandberg (1991) much earlier than the publication of the review by Muheim et al. (2006a).

    The authors of this review suggested that polarized light cues at sunset, SS (and possibly also at sunrise, SR) were essential for magnetic compass recalibration. However, it should be kept in mind that birds do actually not need to see the SS (or SR) point to be able to deduce their position from the sunlight polarization pattern. At SS and SR, the band of maximum polarization (BMP) passes directly through the zenith and, along with the electrical vector of polarized light, is aligned vertically to the horizon (Fig. 1). In contrast to the position of a sunset point, the intersections of the BMP with the horizon at SS and SR are independent of topography. Moreover, partial view of the sky should be sufficient for deducing the SS point from the BMP at sunset or sunrise.

    Figure  1.  Three-dimensional illustration of the band of maximum polarization (BMP) at sunrise and sunset, intersecting the horizon vertically. Course of setting sun in locations with different horizon features, illustrating that the position/timing of the local sunset depends on topography (from Muheim et al., 2007).

    Muheim et al. (2006b, 2007) conducted their cue-conflict experiments in Emlen funnels (Emlen and Emlen, 1966) using wild-caught Savannah Sparrows to test whether these birds recalibrated their magnetic compass relative to the skylight polarization cues. The authors suggested that migrants averaged directional information obtained from skylight polarization patterns at SS and SR to derive a geographic directional system that provides the primary calibration reference for all of the compasses of migratory songbirds (Muheim et al., 2006b). Averaging the directions of SS and SR is an inviting opportunity indeed, because the bisector of the directions towards the SS and SR points is the north-south axis, 365 days a year in any location on the earth. However, the results of Muheim et al. (2006b) do not actually provide evidence for averaging directional information from skylight polarization patterns at SS and SR. The birds shifted their orientation by 90° after being exposed to polarization patterns shifted by 90°, which means that they did not average their orientation, for otherwise the directional shift by the birds should have been 45°, not 90° (as shown by Bill Cochran in a discussion at the RIN Animal Navigation Forum in September 2006).

    Another problem, emphasized by Wiltschko et al. (2008a), is that it is not at all clear whether the Savannah Sparrows, tested by Muheim et al. (2007), were in the migratory disposition in the first place. Neither was the initial distribution of the headings of the birds shown nor was a mean vector given. The sample from which the test birds were selected may not have been oriented towards their migratory direction. The authors selected individuals that oriented to the southeast (the assumed migratory direction) ±90° for further testing. This is a quite problematic procedure, because the birds taken from a sample which is not significantly oriented cannot be assumed to be oriented even if their headings per chance lie in the migratory direction. A second test before exposure to check whether the birds truly headed in their migratory direction was not reported. Therefore, it is not immediately obvious whether the behavior of the birds in this experiment was at all migratory.

    Wiltschko et al. (2008a) obtained contradictory results when testing Australian Silvereyes (Zosterops lateralis) in Emlen funnels. Their birds paid no attention to cue conflict and maintained the same seasonally appropriate migratory direction both before and after exposure. Unlike Muheim et al. (2007) who altered polarization pattern by using polarizers (which is extremely difficult to do in a fashion similar to the natural pattern), Wiltschko et al. (2008a) rotated the magnetic field by Helmholtz coils and let the birds see the naturally polarized sky. Muheim et al. (2008) explained the discrepancy between their results (Muheim et al., 2006b, 2007) and those by Wiltschko et al. (2008a) by the fact that the Australian Silvereyes tested in the latter study could have calibrated migratory compass direction on landmarks they could see in the aviary. However, it is not immediately clear why, if silvereyes in Australia did it as suggested by Muheim et al. (2008), Savannah Sparrows in Alaska studied by Muheim et al. (2006b, 2007) failed to do the same. Summarizing, the claim that Savannah Sparrows calibrated their magnetic compass on skylight polarization cues and, especially that they averaged directions from both SS and SR, seems less than convincing.

    It should be however noted that in later experiments with White-throated Sparrows (Zonotrichia albicollis) Muheim et al. (2009) rotated the magnetic field, similar to Wiltschko et al. (2008a) and obtained the same results (priority of skylight polarization compass) as were recorded for Savannah Sparrows (Muheim et al., 2006b, 2007). Furthermore, White-throated Sparrows tested in spring and in autumn (Muheim et al., 2009) undoubtedly were on migration. Generally, in the latter study the authors addressed some of the concerns voiced by Wiltschko et al. (2008a, 2008b) and their results look more convincing than the results of their earlier work with Savannah Sparrows (Muheim et al., 2006b, 2007).

    Like Wiltschko et al. (2008a), Gaggini et al. (2010) reported that Pied Flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca) did not calibrate their magnetic compass on sunset cues during spring migration in Italy. However, their study is problematic in the same manner as the study by Muheim et al. (2007): the initial sample of untreated birds captured during the spring migration was not significantly oriented in the migratory direction (Fig. 4a in Gaggini et al., 2010). All birds in the Italian study were, beyond doubt, on migration but since they were tested within several days after capture, their behavior may have reflected stress responses rather than migratory direction. Freshly captured migrants, unlike individuals that are given time to get accustomed to captivity, are known often to show phototactic or other stress responses (Marchetti and Baldaccini, 2003). If the birds do not show a meaningful migratory orientation in the first place, all further cue-conflict experiments with them are necessarily problematic.

    In another study performed during autumn migration in Denmark, Pied Flycatchers and Redstarts (Phoenicurus phoenicurus) did not calibrate their magnetic compass from sunset cues (Rabøl, 2010). Their orientation in Emlen funnels during the deep night was not affected by pre-exposure to a horizontally rotated geomagnetic field.

    The study by Cochran et al. (2004) very convincingly showed that the Gray-cheeked Thrush (Catharus minimus) and Swainson's Thrush (C. ustulatus) indeed infer compass direction from sunset cues before take-off and maintain it during nocturnal migratory flight on the basis of their magnetic compass. In this study migrants were not tested in Emlen funnels but were radio-tagged and tracked when flying by natural nocturnal migratory flight. At twilight, these two species of Catharus thrushes were exposed to an artificial magnetic field that was identical to the natural geomagnetic field, but rotated 80° clockwise (Fig. 2). When the birds where released, they immediately found themselves in the natural unaltered magnetic field and flew generally 80° counterclockwise of their normal migratory direction. During the next night, their migratory direction was back to normal, suggesting that compass recalibration occurs every day (Fig. 2). The study by Cochran et al. (2004) is a very convincing one and very clean methodologically. It is difficult to agree with the criticism by Wiltschko et al. (2008a) who claimed that this study is problematic since the birds were free-flying rather than confined to an Emlen funnel. In our opinion, this is a very strong feature of this study and not its drawback. The suggestion that the thrushes of Cochran et al. could be overcompensating after exposure to cue conflict (Wiltschko et al., 2008) does not hold either because, as correctly emphasized by Muheim et al. (2008), these birds showed deflected orientation when tracked immediately upon release just after cue conflict.

    Figure  2.  Predicted orientation responses of birds with respect to potential orientation mechanisms and their interactions. (A) If the stars or the magnetic field show simple domination, or if the stars are calibrated from twilight cues, there should be no effect of the magnetic treatment after release (third and fourth column). (B) If birds use their magnetic compass to calibrate a celestial compass at sunset, they should head east on the first night after treatment and then in their normal northerly migratory direction. (C) If twilight cues are used to calibrate the magnetic compass on a daily basis, birds experiencing a magnetic field turning towards the east, should orient towards the west during the same night after release. On subsequent nights they should return to their normal northerly migratory direction. (A to C) The four thin parallel arrows indicate the horizontal direction of the magnetic field lines experienced by the birds. The thick arrow indicates the expected orientation of the birds. The star indicates the unchanged directional information, potentially available from celestial cues (from Cochran et al., 2004).

    However, a recent study by Chernetsov et al. (2011) aimed to repeat the work of Cochran et al. (2004) in a European medium-distance migrant, the Song Thrush (Turdus philomelos). It was also based on tracking free-flying birds, with the difference that Chernetsov et al. (2011) did not follow the released radio-tagged birds by car, but just recorded their vanishing directions at least 15 km after their nocturnal migratory take-off. Song Thrushes in this study did not calibrate their compass systems, but showed a simple domination of either a magnetic or possibly a stellar compass, as suggested by the lack of any effect of magnetic field horizontal rotation prior to release on their flight directions (Fig. 3).

    Figure  3.  Predicted orientation responses and results of orientation tests performed with song thrushes. (A to C) Predicted responses of magnetically treated birds according to the proposed hypotheses in the fall (the magnetic field deflected 120° clockwise) and spring (the magnetic field deflected 120° anticlockwise), similar to Fig. 2. (D to G) Bearings of song thrushes kept in the ambient magnetic field (spring – D, autumn – F) and in a magnetic field deflected 120° counterclockwise (spring – E) or clockwise (autumn – G) prior to release. D: 26° (n = 7, r = 0.97, p < 0.001); E: 23° (n = 8, r = 0.94, p < 0.001); F: 232° (n = 6, r = 0.95, p < 0.001); G: 249° (n = 5, r = 0.92, p < 0.006). Each dot on the circle periphery represents the bearing of the last point at which the radio signal of the bird was detected. The arrow represents the mean group vector. The inner and outer dashed circles represent the radius of the group mean vector needed for significance (p < 0.05 and p < 0.01, respectively). The two radial dashed lines flanking the group mean vector represent CI (from Chernetsov et al., 2011).

    The studies by Cochran et al. (2004) and Muheim et al. (2006b, 2007, 2009) impressed the avian orientation community greatly and many authors quickly forgot previous results, suggesting the primary role of the magnetic compass (see above). A reason to discard this information was that tracking of free-flying migrants is considered methodologically superior to Emlen funnel tests that, even though broadly used in avian orientation research, still represent a highly unnatural and probably stressful situation for migrants. However, some results suggesting recalibration of celestial cues by magnetic compass were obtained by release experiments, not in Emlen funnels (e.g. Sandberg et al., 2000). Therefore we argue that the very impressive results by Cochran et al. (2004) and Muheim et al. (2006b), providing strong evidence that the two Catharus thrushes and probably Savannah and White-throated Sparrows used skylight polarization pattern as their primary compass cue and regularly recalibrate their magnetic compass on it, should not be immediately generalized to other avian migrants. Even if we believe that the results of Gaggini (2010) suggesting no cue calibration are questionable, we still have the results by Wiltschko et al. (2008a) and Rabøl (2010) obtained by Emlen funnel testing and, rather more importantly, the results from Chernetsov et al. (2011) from free-flying Song Thrushes suggest that some nocturnal migrants do not calibrate compass cues but rely on simple domination.

    It has been suggested by Susanne Åkesson during her talk at the 7th Royal Institute of Navigation 2011 conference "Orientation and Navigation. Birds, Humans and Other Animals" in Reading, UK, that among-species variation observed might be explained by the necessity to cross areas with broadly varying declination values (declination is the angular difference between the magnetic North and the geographic North) in long-distance North American passerine migrants, whereas European migrants and Australian Silvereyes may not face this challenge and remain within the zones with similar inclination values throughout their annual cycle. Another non-excluding possibility mentioned by Chernetsov et al. (2011) is that long-distance migrants generally need a capacity for more precise orientation, whereas short- and medium-distance migrants (e.g. Australian Silvereyes and Song Thrushes) may tolerate larger angular deviations from their inherited compass migratory directions.

  • Aborn DA (2010) Possible competition between waterfowl and Sandhill cranes at Hiwassee wildlife refuge, Tennessee. Proceedings of the North American Crane Workshop, In, pp 15–21
    Ackerman JT, Takekawa JY, Orthmeyer DL, Fleskes JP, Yee JL, Kruse KL (2006) Spatial use by wintering Greater white-fronted geese relative to a decade of habitat change in California's central valley. J Wildlife Manage 70(4):965–976
    Alonso JC, Bautista LM, Alonso JA (2004) Family-based territoriality vs flocking in wintering Common cranes Grus grus. J Avian Biol 35(5):434–444
    Amano T (2012) Unravelling the dynamics of organisms in a changing world using ecological modelling. Ecol Res 27(3):495–507
    Ashmole NP (1963) The regulation of numbers of tropical oceanic birds. Ibis 103b(3):458–473
    Backwell PR, O'hara PD, Christy JH (1998) Prey availability and selective foraging in shorebirds. Anim Behav 55(6):1659–1667
    Baschuk MS, Koper N, Wrubleski DA, Goldsborough G (2012) Effects of water depth, cover and food resources on habitat use of marsh birds and waterfowl in boreal wetlands of Manitoba, Canada. Waterbirds 35(1):44–55
    Beerens JM, Gawlik DE, Herring G, Cook MI (2011) Dynamic habitat selection by two wading bird species with divergent foraging strategies in a seasonally fluctuating wetland. Auk 128(4):651–662
    Bishop MA, Li F (2002) Effects of farming practices in Tibet on wintering Black-necked Crane (Grus nigricollis) diet and food availability. Biodivers Sci 10(4):393–398
    Burger J, Gochfeld M (2013) Wood Storks (Mycteria americana) prey on eggs and hatchlings of Olive ridley sea turtles (Lepidochelys olivacea) at Ostional, Costa Rica. Waterbirds 36(3):358–363
    Cai T, Huettmann F, Guo Y (2014) Using stochastic gradient boosting to infer stopover habitat selection and distribution of Hooded Cranes Grus monacha during spring migration in Lindian, Northeast China. PLoS One 9(2), e89913
    Cao L, Mark B, Zhao M, Meng H, Zhang Y (2011) A systematic scheme for monitoring waterbird populations at Shengjin Lake, China: methodology and preliminary results. Chinese Birds 2(1):1–17
    Chen JY, Zhou LZ, Zhou B, Xu RX, Zhu WZ, Xu WB (2011) Seasonal dynamics of wintering waterbirds in two shallow lakes along Yangtze River in Anhui Province. Zoolog Res 32(5):540–548
    Clausen KK, Clausen P, Fælled CC, Mouritsen KN (2012) Energetic consequences of a major change in habitat use: endangered Brent geese Branta bernicla hrota losing their main food resource. Ibis 154(4):803–814
    Collar NJ, Crosby R, Crosby M (2001) Threatened Birds of Asia: the BirdLife International Red Data Book, vol 1. BirdLife International, Cambridge, UK
    Demment MW, van Soest PJ (1985) A nutritional explanation for body-size patterns of ruminant and nonruminant herbivores. Am Nat 125(5):641–672
    Eadie J, Elphick C, Reinecke K, Miller M, Manley S (2008) Wildlife values of North American ricelands. In: Manley SW (ed) Conservation in Ricelands of North America. The Rice Foundation, Stuttgart, Arkansas, pp 7–90
    Elphick CS, Oring LW (2003) Conservation implications of flooding rice fields on winter waterbird communities. Agr Ecosyst Environ 94(1):17–29
    Emlen JM (1966) The role of time and energy in food preference. Am Nat 100:611–617
    Fauchald P, Erikstad KE, Skarsfjord H (2000) Scale-dependent predator–prey interactions: the hierarchical spatial distribution of seabirds and prey. Ecology 81(3):773–783
    Folmer EO, Olff H, Piersma T (2010) How well do food distributions predict spatial distributions of shorebirds with different degrees of self-organization? J Anim Ecol 79(4):747–756
    Fox AD, Kahlert J, Ettrup H (1998) Diet and habitat use of moulting Greylag geese Anser anser on the Danish island of Saltholm. Ibis 140(4):676–683
    Fox AD, Cao L, Zhang Y, Barter M, Zhao MJ, Meng FJ, Wang SL (2011) Declines in the tuber-feeding waterbird guild at Shengjin Lake National Nature Reserve, China — a barometer of submerged macrophyte collapse. Aquat Conserv 21(1):82–91
    Fretwell S, Lucas HL (1969) On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. Acta Biotheor 19(1):16–36
    Fryxell JM (1991) Forage quality and aggregation by large herbivores. Am Nat 138(2):478–498
    Gyimesi A, Franken MS, Feige N, Nolet BA (2012) Human disturbance of Bewick's swans is reflected in giving-up net energy intake rate, but not in giving-up food density. Ibis 154(4):781–790
    Harris J, Su LY, Higuchi H, Ueta M, Zhang ZW, Zhang YY, Ni XJ (2000) Migratory stopover and wintering locations in eastern China used by White-naped Cranes Grus vipio and Hooded Cranes G. monacha, as determined by satellite tracking. Forktail 16:93–99
    Heaney V, Monaghan P (1996) Optimal allocation of effort between reproductive phases: the trade-off between incubation costs and subsequent brood rearing capacity. P Roy Soc B-Biol Sci 263(1377):1719–1724
    Jia Y, Jiao S, Zhang Y, Zhou Y, Lei G, Liu G (2013) Diet shift and its impact on foraging behavior of Siberian crane (Grus leucogeranus) in Poyang Lake. PLoS One 8(6), e65843
    Jing K, Ma Z, Li B, Li J, Chen J (2007) Foraging strategies involved in habitat use of shorebirds at the intertidal area of Chongming Dongtan, China. Eco Res 22(4):559–570
    Jónsson JE, Afton AD (2006) Different time and energy budgets of Lesser snow geese in rice-prairies and coastal marshes in Southwest Louisiana. Waterbirds 29(4):451–458
    Kuwae T, Miyoshi E, Sassa S, Watabe Y (2010) Foraging mode shift in varying environmental conditions by dunlin Calidris alpina. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 406:281–289
    Lantz SM, Gawlik DE, Cook MI (2011) The effects of water depth and emergent vegetation on foraging success and habitat selection of wading birds in the Everglades. Waterbirds 34(4):439–447
    Li Z, Wang Z, Ge C (2013) Time budgets of wintering Red-crowned cranes: effects of habitat, age and family size. Wetlands 33(2):227–232
    Link PT, Afton AD, Cox RR, Davis BE (2011) Use of habitats by female mallards wintering in southwestern Louisiana. Waterbirds 34(4):429–438
    Liu Q, Yang J, Yang X, Zhao J, Yu H (2010) Foraging habitats and utilization distributions of Black-necked cranes wintering at the Napahai Wetland, China. J Field Ornithol 81(1):21–30
    Liu ZY, Xu WB, Wang QS, Shi KC, Xu JS, Yu GQ (2001) Environmental carrying capacity for overwintering Hooded Cranes in Shenjin Lake. Resources and Environment in the Yangtze Basin 10(5):454–459
    Ma Z, Li B, Jing K, Zhao B, Tang S, Chen J (2003) Effects of tidewater on the feeding ecology of Hooded Crane (Grus monacha) and conservation of their wintering habitats at Chongming Dongtan, China. Ecol Res 18(3):321–329
    MacArthur RH, Pianka ER (1966) On optimal use of a patchy environment. Am Nat 100(916):603–609
    Marra PP, Holberton RL (1998) Corticosterone levels as indicators of habitat quality: Effects of habitat segregation in a migratory bird during the non-breeding season. Oecologia 116:284–292
    Meine CD, Archibald GW (1996) The Cranes: Status Survey and Conservation Action Plan. IUCN, Gland, Switzerland, and Cambridge, U.K.
    Miller M, Garr J, Coates P (2010) Changes in the status of harvested rice fields in the Sacramento Valley, California: implications for wintering waterfowl. Wetlands 30(5):939–947
    Moen AN (1976) Energy conservation by White-tailed deer in the winter. Ecology 57:192–198
    Newton I (1998) Population Limitation in Birds. Academic, San Diego
    Newton I (2004) Population limitation in migrants. Ibis 146(2):197–226
    Nolet BA, Bevan RM, Klaassen M, Langevoord O, van Der Heijden YGJT (2002) Habitat switching by Bewick's swans: maximization of average long-term energy gain? J Anim Ecol 71(6):979–993
    Ohsako Y (1994) Analysis of crane population change, habitat selection, and human disturbance in Japan. In: Higuchi H, Miton J (eds) The Future of Cranes and Wetlands. Wilde Bird Society of Japan, Tokyo, pp 107–113
    Puglisi L, Claudia Adamo M, Emilio Baldaccini N (2005) Man-induced habitat changes and sensitive species: a GIS approach to the Eurasian bittern (Botaurus stellaris) distribution in a Mediterranean wetland. Biodivers Conserv 14(8):1909–1922
    Quaintenne G, Van Gils JA, Bocher P, Dekinga A, Piersma T (2010) Diet selection in a molluscivore shorebird across Western Europe: does it show short- or long-term intake rate- maximization? J Anim Ecol 79(1):53–62
    Reynolds MH (2004) Habitat use and home range of the Laysan Teal on Laysan Island, Hawaii. Waterbirds 27(2):183–192
    Strong AM, Sherry TW (2000) Habitat-specific effects of food abundance on the condition of Ovenbirds wintering in Jamaica. J Anim Ecol 69:883–895
    Thomas DL, Taylor EJ (2006) Study designs and tests for comparing resource use and availability Ⅱ. J Wildlife Manage 70(2):324–336
    Tourenq C, Bennetts RE, Kowalski H, Vialet E, Lucchesi J-L, Kayser Y, Isenmann P (2001) Are ricefields a good alternative to natural marshes for waterbird communities in the Camargue, southern France? Biol Conserv 100(3):335–343
    van Beest FM, Mysterud A, Loe LE, Milner JM (2010) Forage quantity, quality and depletion as scale-dependent mechanisms driving habitat selection of a large browsing herbivore. J Anim Ecol 79(4):910–922
    Wang Q (1988) Hooded Crane (Grus monacha). Chin J Zool 23(4):30–39
    Wang X, Fox AD, Cong P, Cao L (2013) Food constraints explain the restricted distribution of wintering Lesser white-fronted Geese Anser erythropus in China. Ibis 155(3):576–592
    Weimerskirch H, Ancel A, Caloin M, Zahariev A, Spagiari J, Kersten M, Chastel O (2003) Foraging efficiency and adjustment of energy expenditure in a pelagic seabird provisioning its chick. J Anim Ecol 72(3):500–508
    White G, Purps J, Alsbury S (2006) The Bittern in Europe: a Guide to Species and Habitat Management. Royal Society for the Protection of Birds. Sandy, Utah, U.S.
    Xu L, Xu W, Sun Q, Zhou Z, Shen J, Zhao X (2008) Flora and vegetation in Shengjin Lake. J Wuhan Botanical Res 27(3):264–270
    Zhao F, Zhou L, Xu W (2013) Habitat utilization and resource partitioning of wintering Hooded Cranes and three goose species at Shengjin Lake. Chinese Birds 4(4):281–290
    Zhou B, Zhou L, Chen J, Cheng Y, Xu W (2010) Diurnal time-activity budgets of wintering Hooded Cranes (Grus monacha) in Shengjin Lake, China. Waterbirds 33(1):110–115
  • Related Articles

Catalog

    Figures(3)  /  Tables(3)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (261) PDF downloads (29) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return