Dongmei Zhang, Lizhi Zhou, Yunwei Song. 2015: Effect of water level fluctuations on temporal-spatial patterns of foraging activities by the wintering Hooded Crane (Grus monacha). Avian Research, 6(1): 16. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-015-0026-x
Citation: Dongmei Zhang, Lizhi Zhou, Yunwei Song. 2015: Effect of water level fluctuations on temporal-spatial patterns of foraging activities by the wintering Hooded Crane (Grus monacha). Avian Research, 6(1): 16. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-015-0026-x

Effect of water level fluctuations on temporal-spatial patterns of foraging activities by the wintering Hooded Crane (Grus monacha)

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Lizhi Zhou, zhoulz@ahu.edu.cn

  • Received Date: 14 Feb 2015
  • Accepted Date: 21 Jul 2015
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 04 Aug 2015
  • Background 

    The Yangtze River floodplain provides important wintering habitats for Hooded Cranes (Grus monacha) in China. Fluctuations in the water level change foraging habitat and food availability, affecting their temporal-spatial patterns of foraging activities. It is of considerable importance to investigate the effect of these fluctuations on food availability for wintering Hooded Cranes and their foraging response to these changes. Understanding their behavior patterns is beneficial in protecting the wintering crane population and restoring their wintering habitats.

    Methods 

    A field survey of the winter behavior of cranes was carried out at Shengjin Lake from November in 2013 to April in 2014. Habitat variables, as well as the spatial distribution and behavior patterns of wintering cranes at their foraging sites during five stages of water level fluctuation were collected. Based on this data we analyzed the relationship of foraging behavior relative to water level fluctuations and habitat types.

    Results 

    The foraging habitats used by Hooded Cranes varied at the different water level stages. As the water level decreased, the use of meadows and mudflats increased. When the water dropped to its lowest level, the use by the Hooded Crane in the mudflats reached a peak. There were statistically significant differences in time budget in the three types of habitats over the five stages of the water level. In the mudflats, the foraging behavior and maintenance behavior varied significantly with the water level, while the alert behavior showed little variation. Analysis of a generalized linear model showed that the five water level stages and three habitat types had a significant effect on foraging behavior, while the combined effect of these two variables was significant on the foraging time budget and the length of foraging activity of the Hooded Crane.

    Conclusions 

    With the decrease in the water level, the use of mudflats by Hooded Cranes increased correspondingly. Food availability in different habitats was affected by changes in the water level. The Hooded Crane adjusted its foraging patterns and made full use of the three available types of habitat in order to acquire enough food in response to fluctuations in the water level.

  • Avian brood parasitism provides a powerful study system with challenging questions regarding the direction and speed of coevolutionary arms races, including host selection, antagonistic interspecies interactions, and perceptual mimicry (Davies, 2011; Kilner and Langmore, 2011). Obligate brood parasitic birds, including the Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus; hereafter: Cuckoo), and their hosts provide ideal models to study strong reciprocal selection pressures, because parasitism imposes moderate-to-severe costs on host parents that provision unrelated young in their nests and hosts evolve antiparasitic traits (Davies, 2011). Such coevolutionary arms races have resulted in a diverse set of quantifiable differences in the behaviors of both typical and potential host populations that are either in contact, or in isolation, with a brood parasite (Davies and Brooke, 1989; Lahti, 2005; Hale and Briskie, 2007; Stokke et al., 2007; Grim et al., 2011).

    Research effort devoted to brood parasitism in birds has increased dramatically in recent decades (Grim, 2007). However, most studies have focused only on suitable Cuckoo hosts (Davies, 2011), whereas antiparasitic behaviors of rare (Moksnes and Røskaft, 1992; Honza et al., 2004; Procházka and Honza, 2004) or unsuitable (Davies and Brooke, 1989; Moksnes et al., 1991; Grim et al., 2011) hosts have been tested only occasionally. Studying apparently unsuitable Cuckoo hosts provides important insights for our understanding of hostparasite coevolution. For example, variation in the behavioral responses of unsuitable hosts to experimental parasitism can simulate conditions at the start of brood parasitism in a novel (i.e., so far unused) host, and also give information on the phylogenetic distribution of behavioral traits that can serve as pre-adaptations for defenses against brood parasites (Moskát et al., 2003a).

    Species which have some allopatric populations, and other sympatric populations, with brood parasites, provide valuable opportunities to study potential specieslevel flexibility of antiparasitic strategies (Davies and Brooke, 1989; Stokke et al., 2007; Grim et al., 2011). Importantly, almost all studies on the effects of sympatry vs. allopatry with a brood parasite on host responses were conducted in regions for which the length of sympatry or allopatry was unknown, i.e., reliable historical information on how long parasites were absent or cohabiting with hosts is missing in most cases (Davies and Brooke, 1989; Røskaft et al., 2002; Grim et al., 2011). Rare exceptions include the work of Lahti (2005) who knew accurately the length of allopatry in populations of Village Weaverbirds (Ploceus cucullatus) introduced from Africa to the islands of Mauritius and Hispaniola, and Hale and Briskie (2007) who knew the exact length of allopatry in populations of several European avian species introduced to New Zealand (see also Samaš et al., 2011).

    Here, we take advantage of the known length of complete allopatry with Cuckoos in the European Greenfinch (Carduelis chloris; hereafter: Greenfinch); this species was introduced from the United Kingdom (hereafter: UK) to New Zealand (hereafter: NZ) between 1863–1868 (Thomson, 1922). Although less than 100 individuals were introduced, today the Greenfinch is common throughout NZ and there is no genetic evidence for severe inbreeding or genetic drift in this species (Merilä et al., 1996; see also Briskie and MacKintosh, 2004). This is important as bottlenecks could lead to genetic changes and may significantly influence clutch characteristics, behavioral and sensory traits and other aspects of introduced populations (Briskie and MacKintosh, 2004; Congdon and Briskie, 2010). In contrast, several studies have detected divergent changes in the breeding characteristics of the Greenfinch between the source population in the UK and the introduced population in NZ, including lower clutch size (Niethammer, 1970; MacMillan, 1985; Evans et al., 2005) and smaller egg size (Cassey et al., 2005) in NZ. These changes support general observations that passerines in the Northern Hemisphere have higher clutch sizes, fewer breeding attempts per season and/or a shorter breeding season in contrast to the Southern Hemisphere regions (Martin, 1996; Samaš et al., 2013).

    Nevertheless, methodologically independent replication of whole studies (so called "meta-replication") is critically important for the validity and generality of scientific information (Johnson, 2002; Kelly, 2006). Therefore, additionally to testing Greenfinch responses to experimental brood parasitism in allopatry with the Cuckoo, we also studied critical parameters of Greenfinch breeding biology in NZ (clutch and egg sizes). Traits of reproductive investment, including clutch size and egg size, are important components of avian parasite-host arms races because clutch and egg sizes may be related to individual host selection by female parasites and egg rejection decisions by hosts, and clutch size may evolve divergently between host populations sympatric vs. allopatric with parasites (e.g., Soler et al., 2001; Hauber, 2003; Servedio and Hauber, 2006; White et al., 2007). Increase of clutch size might also act as a defensive trait against brood parasites. For example, Svensson and Råberg (2010) and Soler et al. (2011) suggested that individual hosts better tolerate interspecific parasitism by laying larger clutches.

    The Greenfinch is considered an unsuitable host for the Cuckoo because it feeds its nestlings mostly with plant seeds (Davies and Brooke, 1989; Moksnes et al., 1991). However, expectations on host suitability based on nestling diet composition have been shown empirically to be misleading in certain cases (Martín-Gálvez et al., 2005; Grim, 2006). Indeed, there are several documented reports of Cuckoo chicks being successfully raised by the Greenfinch (Seel and Davis, 1981). Clearly, more studies are needed to understand GreenfinchCuckoo interactions in the context of brood parasitism, for example, to test whether flexible foraging and diet selection by Greenfinches, the flexibility of young Cuck oos digestion, or the interaction of both these factors can explain variation in the Cuckoo chick's potential survival in Greenfinch nests (see also Grim et al., 2011).

    No study has specifically tested the behavior of Greenfinch regarding egg rejection behavior in NZ (cf. Boulton and Cassey, 2006; Hale and Briskie, 2007). In addition, only two European studies have reported responses of the Greenfinch to experimental brood parasitism. Davies and Brooke (1989) used three types of hard and solid artificial egg models that were painted to resemble eggs of Cuckoos parasitizing Common Redstarts (Phoenicurus phoenicurus), Pied Wagtails (Motacilla alba) and Meadow Pipits (Anthus pratensis). None of these models introduced during the laying or incubation stages (n = 12) were rejected by Greenfinches (Table 3 in Davies and Brooke, 1989). However, when a Meadow Pipit-like model egg was placed in completed Greenfinch nests before laying began, 2 out of 7 Greenfinches showed rejection (1 deserted, 1 built over; p. 216 in Davies and Brooke, 1989). Thus, the Greenfinch in the UK seems to be capable of responding to foreign eggs in the nest at least in some circumstances.

    Elsewhere, Moksnes et al. (1991) reported that 41% Greenfinches (n = 17) rejected non-mimetic model eggs when introduced at both laying and incubation stages. Rejections (n = 7; Moksnes et al., 1991) were mostly by desertion (n = 5) which may or may not have been a specific response to the appearance of a foreign egg (Kosciuch et al., 2006; see also Results of the present study). Other responses were described as "unselective ejection" (n = 2) which means that "one, several, or all of the host's eggs were destroyed or removed. In the majority of such cases, the artificial egg was left in the nest" (p. 350 in Moksnes et al., 1991). Such "unselective ejection" would be difficult to distinguish from the impacts of nest predators, especially if all natural, host eggs disappear or are destroyed (see Weidinger, 2010). Furthermore, as both of these previous studies were conducted without a control non-manipulated nestor clutch-treatment, it remains unclear whether tested Greenfinches responded specifically to brood parasite eggs or non-specifically to a general clutch disturbance (see Kosciuch et al., 2006).

    Here we experimentally tested egg rejection behavior in an introduced Greenfinch population in NZ. In addition, we compared egg and clutch sizes of this introduced population and their European source population. We predicted that the Greenfinch in NZ would not discriminate against alien eggs (Davies and Brooke, 1989), and show smaller clutch sizes and/or smaller egg sizes in NZ in the southern hemisphere than in northern UK source populations (Cassey et al., 2005).

    All breeding and experimental data were collected during October and November 2007–2008 in the city of Hamilton in NZ and its rural vicinity (37°46'S, 175°16' E). We searched for Greenfinch nests in shrub vegetation and in plantations of blueberry (Vaccinium sp.) orchards. Individuals were not ringed for identification in this study. We used each nest as an independent data point in the statistical analyses.

    We checked nests (n = 41) every 1–5 days. We marked all eggs (n = 158) on the blunt pole for individual recognition. After clutch completion we measured egg length and width by digital calliper to the nearest 0.01 mm.

    For Greenfinches, the grasp or puncture ejection of hard artificial model may represent a problem due to beak size and geometry (Martín-Vivaldi et al., 2002). Therefore, we did not use hard plastic models but painted one randomly selected own host egg with a nontoxic SharpieTM felt tip black pen, following methods of Hauber et al. (2006), so that the experimental design allowed the "foreign" eggs to be puncture ejected. We painted the eggs black given that the natural colour of Greenfinch eggs is greyish-white to bluish-white, or beige, sparsely spotted dark reddish, concentrated at the blunt end (Cramp, 1994; see Fig. 1b in Boulton and Cassey, 2006). Thus, our experimental manipulation of the egg phenotype caused a large change in the host egg's appearance (chroma, brightness, and maculation) to provide a strong test of host egg rejection abilities compared to the potential use of mimetic experimental eggs (Grim, 2005). After painting the eggs we checked that there were no cracks in the eggshells that also might cause egg ejection or nest desertion and did not find any such cracks. Importantly, our manipulation did not alter the overall clutch size of the potential host (similarly to natural Cuckoo behavior of removing a host egg and replacing it with a parasitic egg; Davies, 2011). Egg painting had no effect on painted eggs' hatchability or chick survival at acceptor Greenfinch nests in our study population. Specifically, all painted eggs successfully hatched, chicks grew normally and we did not find any single missing chicks in manipulated nests (if chicks disappeared, it was always a whole brood, i.e., consequence of nest predation, see Weidinger, 2010).

    Figure  1.  Overview of clutch size estimates (mean ±S.E.) from source European populations (UK) and introduced New Zealand (NZ) populations of the Greenfinch. NZ data with years corresponds to Niethammer (1970), MacMillan (1985), Evans et al. (2005), this study and UK data to Cramp et al. (1994) and Robinson (2005).

    We considered host reaction as an "acceptance" if the manipulated egg was still found in an active nest on the 6th day, or later (Davies and Brooke, 1989; Moksnes et al., 1991). If a manipulated egg disappeared within the 6-day period, but hosts continued incubating the clutch, we considered host reaction as "ejection". The presence of traces of yolk at remaining host eggs was considered as "puncture ejection" of ejected egg (following Moksnes et al., 1994; Antonov et al., 2006). Non-predated nests with eggs found to be unincubated (cold) during several consecutive nest checks were scored as "deserted" (Kosciuch et al., 2006). Finally, depredated nests were excluded from analyses.

    Nest desertion may result from many causes (e.g., inclement weather, human disturbance, predator visit; Weidinger, 2010) and, thus, not always be a specific response to parasitism (Kosciuch et al., 2006). Therefore, it is also necessary to study host behavior at non-parasitized, control nests. Overall, we randomly assigned 20 nests as controls and another 20 nests as experimental treatments. In one additional nest we knew clutch size after clutch completion but the nest was predated during 4th day of experiment. We included this nest in analyses of clutch and egg sizes but had to exclude it from egg experimental treatment analyses, hence sample sizes differ between analyses. During the laying stage, we found and monitored three experimental and 10 control nests without predation, resulting in lower sample sizes for some specific analyses (see also Discussion).

    Except for the egg phenotype manipulation, all nests were treated in the same way. We painted the experimental host own egg once the nest found active with two or more eggs. Painting lasted ~1 minute. Eggs were measured after clutch completion and were returned to nests immediately after measurement. During each nest visit we used an inspection mirror to check nest content and pulled out a random egg from a nest to check the egg temperature by touch (warm, i.e., incubated, or cold, i.e., deserted).

    We obtained UK clutch size data for the Greenfinch from the national record card schemes organized by the BTO (Robinson, 2005) and egg sizes from Cramp et al. (1994), particularly for the UK endemic subspecies (Carduelis chloris harrisoni). To avoid pseudoreplication in egg length and width analyses, we used clutch averages as units of analysis (Cassey et al., 2005). We used Welsch's t-test for unequal variances in statistical testing of mean differences of clutch and egg sizes between NZ and UK (see Ruxton, 2006).

    Results are shown as mean ±S.D. Statistical analyses were performed in JMP 8.0.1 (SAS Institute, 2009).

    Data pooled across both breeding stages (laying + incubation) showed the same desertion rates of 20% (n = 4 out of 20) in both control and experimental nests. For control nests, there was no statistically significant difference in desertion rates between laying (3 out of 10) and incubation stages (1 out of 10; Fisher's exact test, twotailed, p = 0.31). The analogous comparison of experimental nests between laying (2 out of 3) and incubation stages (2 out of 17) also showed no statistical difference in desertion rates (Fisher's exact test, two-tailed, p = 0.09).

    For the laying stage, desertion rates were similar in control (3 out of 10) and experimental nests (2 out of 3; Fisher's exact test, two-tailed, p = 0.51). Similarly, desertion rates during incubation were statistically similar between control (1 out of 10) and experimental nests (2 out of 17; Fisher's exact test, two-tailed, p = 1.00). Within experimental nests, clutch sizes at deserted nests were statistically similar as those at nests where Greenfinches either accepted or ejected the experimental egg (Table 1). Within control nests, clutch sizes did not differ between deserted and non-deserted nests (Table 1).

    Table  1.  Clutch size (mean ±S.E.) of New Zealand introduced Greenfinches according to their behaviour at experimental and control nests. Differences within the experimental nest category were tested with Kruskal-Wallis test, all other tests were Welsch's t-tests for unequal variances (as recommended by Ruxton, 2006). Sample sizes slightly differ from those in Table 2 because of missing data for one experimental and one control nest.
    Response Experimental n Control n t p
    Accepted 4.76 ±0.12 13 4.38 ±0.13 16 -2.26 0.03
    Deserted 5.00 ±0.00 3 4.67 ±0.33 3 -1.00 0.42
    Ejected 5.00 ±0.00 3 n.a. n.a.
    χ2= 1.56 p = 0.46 t = 0.82 p = 0.48
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    At 2 out of 20 nests owners ejected painted eggs and in 1 case ejected own unpainted egg while the painted egg remained in nest (i.e., a case of rejection error, see Discussion; Table 2). Thus, hosts at 3 out of 20 nests showed egg ejection behavior. All 3 ejections were presumably by puncture ejection as evidenced by traces of yolk on the remaining eggs. Ejections occurred 1 to 3 days after the start of experiment (i.e., manipulation of the egg phenotype), respectively. Ejection rates at experimental (3 out of 20 nests) and control nests (0 out of 20) were not statistically different (Fisher's exact test, two-tailed, p = 0.23). Ejection rates at experimental nests did not differ between laying (1 out of 3 nests) and incubation stages (2 out of 17 nests; Fisher's exact test, two-tailed, p = 0.40).

    Table  2.  Overview of New Zealand introduced Greenfinch responses to experimental parasitism (1 host own egg painted black) and control nest treatments. In all, we recorded 3 ejections, 2 cases of "selective ejection" (removal of painted egg) and 1 case of "unselective ejection" (rejection error; sensu Moksnes et al., 1991).
    Response Experimental (n = 20) Control (n = 20)
    Accepted 13 16
    Deserted 4 4
    Ejected 3 0
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Clutch sizes were similar at deserted experimental and control nests (Table 1). Clutch sizes statistically differed (~0.4 eggs) between experimental and control nests that showed "acceptance" (i.e., neither desertion nor ejection response; Table 1).

    Clutch size in our NZ population (4.63 ±0.49, n = 41) did not differ significantly from UK source population (4.72 ±0.74, n = 3438; t = 1.23, df = 47, p = 0.23; Fig. 1). Egg length (mm) in NZ population (20.3 ±0.7, n = 41 nests) was similar to the UK population (20.6 ±0.9, n = 100 nests; t = 1.91, df = 139, p = 0.16). Similarly, egg width (mm) in NZ (14.8 ±0.4, n = 41 nests) did not differ from the UK source population (14.8 ±0.7, n = 100 nests; t = 0.00, df = 139, p = 1.00).

    We provide support for the ability of Greenfinches to puncture eject eggs from nests with simulated parasitic eggs. No such ejection events were reported in control nests. Rates of egg ejection did not statistically differ between experimental and control treatments but this can be explained by joint effects of relatively modest sample sizes and species-specifically low rates of egg ejection (both when unselective ejection is included (15%) or excluded (10%) from our sample of experimental nests). Therefore, this statistical non-significance does not imply biological non-significance.

    Previous studies with the Greenfinch used hard plastic models that cannot be punctured (Martín-Vivaldi et al., 2002) and all such eggs were accepted (Davies and Brooke, 1989) or some were rejected by desertion or remained in the nest with hosts own eggs destroyed (Moksnes et al., 1991). In contrast, our findings suggest that nest desertion by the Greenfinch might not be a behavioral response to foreign eggs in the nest because it occurred at statistically similar frequencies in both experimental and control nests (see also Kosciuch et al., 2006). Our comparisons of nest desertions between control and experimental nests are based on typical sample sizes in this line of research (n = 20 per treatment) but some other specific results should be interpreted more carefully. Sample sizes are particularly low for comparisons of (1) experimental nests between laying (n = 3) and incubation stages (n = 17) and (2) nest desertion between experimental (n = 3) and control nests during the laying stage (n = 10). Our results suggest that nest desertion was not a specific response to parasitism, because it occurred in both control and experimental nests and at similar rates, but additional data from this and other Greenfinch populations would make for a stronger test of the importance of nest desertion as a potential antiparasitic behavior.

    We detected two cases of selective egg ejection of experimentally dyed host eggs and one "unselective ejection" (sensu Moksnes et al., 1991) of own egg (i.e., rejection error), suggesting that Greenfinches are capable of behavioral responses to foreign eggs. Decisions to accept, eject, or desert were not related to clutch size (Table 2). Ejections of manipulated black eggs were most likely an antiparasitism responses because (1) no eggs were ejected at control nests, and (2) hosts at experimental nests specifically targeted the manipulated eggs. In one case a host non-experimental egg was removed by host while the painted egg remained in nest. In a previous study, Boulton and Cassey (2006) used tethered artificial eggs at active nests to identify predators of some European species introduced to NZ, including the Greenfinch. In that study, Greenfinches ejected experimental egg at 1 out of 6 nests providing additional support to our conclusions.

    The observed ability of egg ejection and discrimination in the Greenfinch might have evolved due to several selection pressures. (1) Cuckoos in native UK populations may have provided weak but persistent selection pressure for host defenses against interspecific parasites in the Greenfinch. Even though the Greenfinch has traditionally been considered an unsuitable Cuckoo host, some Cuckoo chicks are known to successfully fledge from Greenfinch nests (Seel and Davis, 1981). Although Cuckoos were not introduced to NZ, in some cases antiparasitism adaptations may persist even after the original selection pressures from interspecific parasites ceased (Rothstein, 2001; Hale and Briskie, 2007; Samaš et al., 2011). However, it is important to highlight that the Greenfinch (a) is rare and unsuitable Cuckoo host, (b) has had its NZ population for ~150 years isolated from European Cuckoo, and (c) is not regular host of any native NZ cuckoos (no evidence for NZ cuckoos parasitism on Greenfinches found in literature). Therefore, alternatively or additionally, (2) parasitism by conspecifics may select for an ability to recognize and reject alien eggs (López-de-Hierro and Moreno-Rueda, 2010). Importantly, conspecific brood parasitism is known in the native ranges of Greenfinch (Yom-Tov, 2000). Despite no longer experiencing potential cuckoo parasitism, NZ Greenfinches would likely not escape conspecific parasites and so the conspecific egg rejection hypothesis may provide a viable explanation for persistence of egg rejection behavior (for a similar case of Turdus thrushes see Samaš et al., 2011). We did not find evidence for conspecific parasitism in NZ Greenfinches in literature but detection of such parasitism is much more difficult than the detection of interspecific parasitism (Latif et al., 2006). (3) Also, nest cleaning/ sanitation (sensu Moskát et al., 2003b; Guigueno and Sealy, 2009) might have contributed to the ejection of the dark-painted foreign eggs in our study. However, this hypothesis can explain only non-specific removal of debris (i.e., non-egg-shaped objects) from nests (Ortega and Cruz, 1988; Moskát et al., 2003b; Guigueno and Sealy, 2009) but does not explain specific ability to reject dissimilar eggs (and egg shaped objects) in birds in general (Grim, 2005).

    This study emphasized the importance of including control nests for assessing a role of nest desertion in antiparasitic defenses (see also Kosciuch et al., 2006). Our study population showed relatively high rates of desertion of experimentally parasitized nests (20%, 4 out of 20) in comparison to data from source European population (0%, n = 12; Table 3 in Davies and Brooke, 1989). In turn, behavior of the NZ Greenfinch at control nests revealed the same rates of desertion (20%, 4 out of 20) as those at experimental nests. Therefore, nest desertion may not be a specific response to experimental parasitism in our study species.

    We did not find significantly different clutch sizes or egg sizes between NZ and UK Greenfinch populations. These results are in contrast with previous studies that showed significantly smaller clutches (Niethammer, 1970; MacMillan, 1985; Evans et al., 2005) and eggs (Cassey et al., 2005) in introduced NZ passerine populations, including Greenfinch. Differences between previous and our work may be caused by different samples used for comparisons.

    Importantly, we found that Greenfinches were able to eject foreign eggs although they also may make recognition errors in egg discrimination. That this was observed in a population allopatric from Cuckoos for a long time, and because Greenfinches are rarely parasitized by Cuckoos in their native ranges, suggests that conspecific parasitism, instead of interspecific parasitism, may be responsible for the functional benefit of egg discrimination by Greenfinches (see also Grim et al., 2011; Samaš et al., 2011).

    Overall, this study underlies the importance of (1) control nests in the study of brood parasitism (Kosciuch et al., 2006; Petrie et al., 2009), (2) studying also seemingly unsuitable hosts of interspecific brood parasites (Moksnes and Røskaft, 1992; Grim et al., 2011), and (3) meta-replication in behavioral ecology (Johnson, 2002; Kelly, 2006).

    We are grateful to D. Hanley, A. Moksnes, E. Røskaft and an anonymous referee for their helpful comments. This study was supported by Human Frontier Science Program awards (RGY69/07 to TG, PC, and MEH and RGY86/12 to TG and MEH). Our research followed local and institutional guidelines for the treatment of research animals. PC is an ARC Future Fellow.

  • Beerens JM, Gawlik DE, Herring G, Cook MI (2011) Dynamic habitat selection by two wading bird speices with divergent foraging strategies in a seasonally fluctuation wetland. Auk 128(4):651-662
    Marra PP, Hobson KA, Holmes RT (1998) Linking winter and summer events in a migratory bird by using stable carbon isotopes. Science 282:1884-1886
    May R, Mclean A (2010) Theoretical ecology. Oxford University Press, Oxford
    Kuwae T, Miyoshi E, Sassa S, Watabe Y (2010) Foraging mode shift in varying environmental conditions by dunlin Calidris alpina. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 406:281-289
    Farago S, Hangya K (2012) Effects of water level on waterbird abundance and diversity along the middle section of the Danube River. Hydrobiologia 697(1):15-21
    Kushlan JA (1978) Nonrigorous foraging by robbing egrets. Ecology 59(4):649-653
    Maheswaran G, Rahmani AR (2001) Effects of water level changes and wading bird abundance on the foraging behavior of black necked storks Ephippiorhynchus asiaticus in Dudwa National Park, India. J Biosci 26(3):373-382
    Lantz SM, Gawlik DE, Cook MI (2010) The effects of water depth and submerged aquatic vegetation on the selection of foraging habitat and foraging success of wading birds. Condor 112(3):460-469
    Master TL, Lesier JK, Bennett KA (2005) Patch selection by Snowy Egrets. Colon Waterbird 28:220-224
    Lewis TL, Esler D, Boyd WS (2008) Foraging behavior of Surf Scoters (Melanitta perspicillata) and White-Winged Scoters (M. fusca) in relation to clam density: inferring food availability and habitat quality. Auk 125(1):149-157
    Loftus WF, Eklund AM (1994) Long-term dynamics of an Everglades small-fish assemblage. In: Davis SM, Ogden JC (eds) Everglades: the ecosystem and its restoration. CRC Press, Boca Raton, FL, pp 461-483
    Russell GJ, Bass OL Jr, Pimm SL (2002) The effect of hydrological patterns and breeding-season flooding on the numbers and distribution of wading birds in Everglades National Park. Anim Conserv 5:185-199
    Baschuk MS, Koper N, Wrubleski DA, Goldsborough G (2012) Effects of water depth, cover and food resources on habitat use of marsh birds and waterfowl in boreal wetlands of Manitoba, Canada. Waterbirds 35(1):44-55
    Holm TE, Clausen P (2006) Effects of water level management on autumn staging waterbird and macrophyte diversity in three Danish coastal lagoons. Biodiv Conserv 15:4399-4423
    Bancroft GT, Gawlik DE, Rutchey K (2002) Distribution of wading birds relative to vegetation and water depths in the northern Everglades of Florida, USA. Waterbirds 25:265-277
    Jing K, Ma ZJ, Li B, Li JH, Chen JK (2007) Foraging strategies involved in habitat use of shorebirds at the intertidal area of Chongming Dongtan, China. Ecol Res 22:559-570
    Beauchamp G (2006) Spatial, temporal and weather factors influencing the foraging behavior of migrating Semipalmated Sandpipers. Waterbirds 29(2):221-225
    Pienkowski MW (1983) Surface activity of some intertidal invertebrates in relation to temperature and the foraging behaviour of their shorebird predators. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 11:141-150
    Zhao F, Zhou L, Xu W (2013) Habitat utilization and resource partitioning of wintering Hooded Cranes and three goose species at Shengjin Lake. Chin Birds 4:281-290
    Cao L, Barter M, Zhao MJ, Meng HX, Zhang Y (2011) A systematic scheme for monitoring waterbird populations at Shengjin Lake, China: methodology and preliminary results. Chin Birds 2:1-17
    Liu ZY, Xu WB, Wang QS, Shi KC, Xu JS, Yu GQ (2001) Environmental carrying capacity for over wintering Hooded Cranes in Shengjin Lake. Resour Envir Yangtze Basin 10(5):454-459
    Cao L, Fox AD (2009) Birds and people both depend on China's wetlands. Nature 460:173
    BirdLife International (2001) Threatened birds of Asia: the BirdLife international red data book. BirdLife International, Cambridge, pp 1174-1179
    Yang GS, Ma GH, Zhang L, Jiang JH, Yao SC, Zhang M et al (2010) Lake status, major problems and protection strategy in China. J Lake Sci 22(6):799-810
    Fox AD, Cao L, Zhang Y, Barter M, Zhao MJ, Meng FJ et al (2011) Declines in the tuber-feeding waterbird guild at Shengjin Lake National Nature Reserve, China—a barometer of submerged macrophyte collapse. Aquatic Conserv Mar Freshw Ecosyst 21:82-91
    Li ZQ, Wang Z, Ge C (2013) Time budgets of wintering Red-Crowned Cranes: effects of habitat, age and family size. Wetlands 33:227-232
    Wood C, Qiao Y, Li P, Ding P, Lu BZ, Xi YM (2010) Implications of rice agriculture for wild birds in China. Waterbirds 33(Sp1):30-43
    Aviles JM (2003) Time budget and habitat use of the Common Crane wintering in dehesas of southwestern Spain. Can J Zool 81:1233-1238
    Erwin RM (1983) Feeding habitats of nesting wading birds: spatial use and social influences. Auk 100:960-970
    BirdLife International (2012) Grus monacha. The IUCN red list of threatened species. Version 2015.2. . Accessed 2 Aug 2015
    Jiao SW, Guo YM, Falk H, Lei GC (2014) Nest-site selection analysis of Hooded Crane (Grus monacha) in Northeastern China based on a multivariate ensemble model. Zoolog Sci 31:430-437
    Luo ZJ (2012) Habitat selection of Hooded Crane (Grus monacha) wintering at the lakes of Yangtze River Floodplain in Anhui Province. Master's Thesis. Anhui University
    Fox AD, Hearn RD, Cao L (2008) Preliminary observations of diurnal feeding patterns of swan geese (Anser cygnoides) using two different habitats at Shengjin Lake, Anhui Province, China. Wildfowl 58:20-30
    James H (2000) Migratory stopover and wintering locations in eastern China used by White-naped Cranes Grus vipio and Hooded Cranes G. monacha as determined by satellite tracking. Forktail 16:93-99
    Zhou B, Zhou LZ, Chen JY, Cheng YQ, Xu WB (2010) Diurnal time-activity budgets of wintering Hooded cranes (Grus monacha) in Shengjin Lake, China. Waterbirds 33(1):110-115
    Xu LL, Xu WB, Sun QY, Zhou ZZ, Shen J, Zhao XX (2008) Flora and vegetation in Shengjin Lake. J Wuhan Botan Res 27(3):264-270
    Yang XL (2011) Research on the numbers, distribution, feeding behavior and diet of Great White Fronted Geese at Shengjin Lake, the national natural reserves in Anhui Province. Master's Thesis. University of Science and Technology of China
    Ma ZJ, Li B, Jing K, Zhao B, Tang SM, Chen JK (2003) Effects of tidewater on the feeding ecology of Hooded Crane (Grus monacha) and conservation of their wintering habitats at ChongmingDongtan, China. Ecol Res 18:321-329
    Zhao C, Li YH, Hu J, Yao G, He Q, Li HY et al (2012) Feeding sites selection of long-billed plover in winter at the middle reaches of Jialing River. Sichuan J Zool 31(1):22-26
    Jia YF (2013) Impact of water level fluctuation on Siberian Crane and other wintering waterbirds in Poyang Lake. PhD Thesis. Beijing Forestry University, Beijing
    Lamoot I (2004) Foraging behaviour and habitat use of large herbivores in a coastal dune landscape. PhD Thesis. Universiteit Gent, Gent
    Azevedo CS, Ferraz JB, Tinoco HP, Young RJ, Rodrigues M (2010) Time-activity budget of greater rheas (Rhea Americana, Aves) on a human-disturbed area: the role of habitat, time of the day, season and group size. Acta Ethol 13:109-117
    Gyimesi A, Franken MS, Feige N, Nolet BA (2012) Human disturbance of Bewick's Swans is reflected in giving-up net energy intake rate, but not in giving-up food density. Ibis 154(4):781-790
    Lafever KE (2006) Spatial and temporal winter territory use and behavioral responses of Whooping Cranes to human activities. Doctoral Dissertation. Texas A & M University
    Coops H, Beklioglu M, Crisman TL (2003) The role of water-level fluctuations in shallow lake ecosystems—workshop conclusions. Hydrobiologia 506-509:23-27
    Wantzen KM, Rothhaupt KO, Mortl M, Cantonati M, Toth LG, Fischer P (2008) Ecological effects of water-level fluctuations in lakes: an urgent issue. Hydrobiologia 613:1-4
    Bolduc F, Afton AD (2008) Monitoring waterbird abundance in wetlands: the importance of controlling results for variation in water depth. Ecol Model 216(3):402-408
    Dimalexis A, Pyrovetsi M (1997) Effect of water level fluctuations on wading bird foraging habitat use at an Irrigatio Reservoir, Lake Kerkini, Greece. Colon Waterbird 20(2):244-252
    Rajpar MN, Zakaria M (2011) Effects of water level fluctuation on waterbirds distribution and aquatic vegetation composition at natura wetland reserve, Peninsular Malaysia. ISRN Ecol 2011:1-13
    Li YK, Shan JH, Ma JZ, Miu LJ, Li J, Yuan FK et al (2014) The effects of climate and water level fluctuation on the wintering population of Oriental White Stork. Chin J Ecol 33(4):1061-1067
    Kushlan JA (1986) Responses of wading birds to seasonally fluctuating water levels: strategies and their limits. Colon Waterbird 2(9):155-162
    Krebs JR (1978) Optimal foraging: decision rules for predators. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 23-63
    Macarthur R, Recher H, Cody M (1966) On the relation between habitat selection and species diversity. Amer Natur 100:319-332
    Stephens DW (1986) Foraging theory. Princeton University Press, Princeton
    Paulus SL (1988) Time-activity budgets of nonbreeding Anatidae: a review. In: Weller MW (ed) Waterfowl in winter. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, pp 135-152
    Woodin MC, Michot TC (2006) Foraging behavior of redheads (Aythya americana) wintering in Texas and Louisiana. Hydrobiologia 567:129-141
    Lee SD, Jabtonski PG, Higuchi H (2007) Winter foraging of threatened cranes in the Demilitarized Zone of Korea: behavioral evidence for the conservation importance of unplowed rice fields. Biol Conserv 138:286-289
    Jing K, Tang SM, Chen JK, Ma ZJ (2002) Primary research on the characteristics of feeding sites of Grus monacha in the east tide flat of Chongming. Zool Res 23(1):84-88
    Riis T, Hawes I (2002) Relationships between water level fluctuations and vegetation diversity in shallow water of New Zealand lakes. Aquat Bot 74:133-148
    Zheng M (2014) The effects of food resource dynamics on foraging behaviors of wintering Hooded Cranes (Grus monacha) over spatio-temporal scales. Master's Thesis. Anhui University
    Aborn DA (2010) Behavior and habitat use of Greater Sandhill Cranes wintering in east Tennessee. Crane Workshop 11:9-14
    Slobodkin LB (1974) Prudent predation does not require group selection. Amer Natur 108:665-678
    Coops H, Hosper SH (2002) Water-level management as a tool for the restoration of Shallow Lake in the Netherlands. Lake Reserv Manag 18(4):293-298
    Gawlik DE (2006) The role of wildlife science in wetland ecosystem restoration: lessons from the Everglades. Ecol Eng 26:70-83
    Zhu WZ, Zhou LZ (2010) Biodoversity and conservation in Anqing floodplain wetlands. Hefei University of Technology Press, Hefei
  • Related Articles

Catalog

    Figures(2)  /  Tables(2)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (240) PDF downloads (14) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return