Zhenhua Wei, Lizhi Zhou. 2023: The impact of earlier flood recession on metacommunity diversity of wintering waterbirds at shallow lakes in the middle and lower Yangtze River floodplain. Avian Research, 14(1): 100102. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2023.100102
Citation: Zhenhua Wei, Lizhi Zhou. 2023: The impact of earlier flood recession on metacommunity diversity of wintering waterbirds at shallow lakes in the middle and lower Yangtze River floodplain. Avian Research, 14(1): 100102. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2023.100102

The impact of earlier flood recession on metacommunity diversity of wintering waterbirds at shallow lakes in the middle and lower Yangtze River floodplain

Funds: This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (32171530) and the Scientific and Technology Project of the Anhui Provincial Group Limited for Yangtze-to-Huaihe Water Diversion (YJJH-ZT-ZX-20180404062)
More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    School of Resources and Environmental Engineering, Anhui University, Hefei, 230601, China. E-mail address: zhoulz@ahu.edu.cn (L. Zhou)

  • Received Date: 15 Feb 2023
  • Rev Recd Date: 17 Apr 2023
  • Accepted Date: 21 Apr 2023
  • Available Online: 14 Jun 2023
  • Publish Date: 23 Apr 2023
  • The hydrological regime in wetlands plays an important role in the process of wintering waterbird metacommunity assemblage. However, increasing frequency of extreme climate and the intensification of human activities, such as the construction of sluices and dams, have resulted in frequently abnormal hydrological regime in the middle and lower Yangtze River floodplain. In recent years, earlier flood recession has become one of the main hydrological problems faced in the shallow lakes, having a great impact on wetland biodiversity. It is necessary to understand the impact of earlier flood recession on waterbirds, an indicator of wetland biodiversity, and the metacommunity concept is helpful to elucidate the underlying mechanism involved in the processes of assemblage by waterbird communities. In this study, we surveyed the wintering waterbirds at three sub-lakes of Caizi Lakes during 2019–2020 and 2020–2021 and compared the richness, abundance, alpha and beta diversity of waterbirds in and among local metacommunities under earlier flood recession and normal hydrological regime. The results showed that the earlier flood recession reduced the species richness in the early stage and abundance in the late stage, it also reduced the Shannon–Wiener index in the early stage and increased the dissimilarity between and within waterbird metacommunities in the late stage. The partition of beta diversity showed that the turnover component played a major role in the process of waterbird metacommunity assemblage. It was found that the earlier flood recession reduced the richness, abundance in different stages of flood recession, which also increased the turnover of waterbirds. Metacommunities with high habitat heterogeneity had better resistance to abnormal hydrological regime, which resulted in high dissimilarity between and within metacommunities. The results of this study provide important information for waterbird conservation and water level management at shallow lakes in the middle and lower Yangtze River floodplain.

  • Avian migration is a spectacular phenomenon that has fascinated humans for centuries. However, many aspects of avian migration biology and particularly of navigation still remain unresolved (Alerstam, 2006). Billions of birds annually move between their breeding and non-breeding areas and many of them show remarkable natal, breeding and wintering site fidelity (Sokolov, 1997; Newton, 2008). To do this, they obviously have to be able to select and maintain flight directions (i.e. to perform migratory orientation) and to find the goal of their movements without any direct sensory contact (i.e. to navigate).

    Migrating birds are able to use an array of compass cues for orientation and complex interactions between different cues apparently exist. In recent years, significant advances have been made in understanding compass mechanisms that migrating birds use to determine and maintain direction of their movements (for a review see Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 2009). Particularly, considerable progress has been achieved in understanding the physiological basis of magnetoreception. The birds seem to possess two independent magnetoreception systems, one in their upper beak (Fleissner et al., 2003, 2007; Falkenberg et al., 2010) and one in the retina of their eye (Mouritsen et al., 2004; Mouritsen and Ritz, 2005; Ritz et al., 2010). Only the system localized in the retina is used as a compass, whereas the upper beak magnetoreceptor is not necessary for orientation (Zapka et al., 2009) and might be responsible for the magnetic map sense. The compass system of migrating birds is apparently redundant, i.e. they use several different compass cues, including celestial, magnetic and possibly also olfactory ones and somehow integrate information that they obtain from these cues. How exactly migrants integrate these cues, how and when they calibrate them and which compass system has priority over others is a subject of debate.

    It has been suggested that the relative importance of celestial and magnetic cues varies between the pre-migratory and migratory periods (Wiltschko et al., 1998a, 1998b; Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1999). During the pre-migratory period, celestial cues provide birds with their primary calibration reference; the magnetic compass is recalibrated when birds are exposed to a rotating magnetic field in the presence of natural celestial cues (Prinz and Wiltschko, 1992; Weindler and Liepa, 1999). Conversely, in most experiments during migration, birds exposed to a rotating magnetic field showed a corresponding shift in the direction of migratory orientation, indicating a precedence of magnetic cues (Wiltschko et al., 1998b, 1999; Sandberg et al., 2000). When tested for after-effects under natural celestial cues in the absence of magnetic cues (vertical magnetic field), the birds maintained the shifted direction, suggesting recalibration of their celestial compass relative to the magnetic field (Bingman and Wiltschko, 1988; Wiltschko et al., 1998b, 1999, 2001; Sandberg et al., 1991, 2000). In the study by Åkesson et al. (2002), juvenile White-crowned Sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys gambelii) showed orientation following the rotation of the magnetic field (90° counterclockwise) even when tested in the natural magnetic field (not in the vertical magnetic field) after exposure to cue-conflict, which did not occur at sunset in that study.

    The only study that contradicted this apparently coherent pattern was that by Able and Able (1995), who showed that both first-autumn and adult Savannah Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis) adjusted their migratory direction as if they were calibrating their magnetic compass relative to celestial cues, not vice versa, as in other studies.

    However, more recently Muheim et al. (2006a) reviewed a large body of evidence and detected another pattern. These authors found that responses to cue conflict, either before the onset of migration or during migration, were correlated with access to celestial cues. Birds exposed to cue conflict during sunset used sunset information as the primary cue if they had an unobstructed view of the entire sky, including its part near the horizon. Conversely, if cue conflict experiments were conducted at times of day that did not include sunset or the sky could not be seen near the horizon, the birds would use the geomagnetic field as their primary compass cue. It should be noted that the importance of seeing the sky near the horizon for the correct use of celestial cues had been suggested by Sandberg (1991) much earlier than the publication of the review by Muheim et al. (2006a).

    The authors of this review suggested that polarized light cues at sunset, SS (and possibly also at sunrise, SR) were essential for magnetic compass recalibration. However, it should be kept in mind that birds do actually not need to see the SS (or SR) point to be able to deduce their position from the sunlight polarization pattern. At SS and SR, the band of maximum polarization (BMP) passes directly through the zenith and, along with the electrical vector of polarized light, is aligned vertically to the horizon (Fig. 1). In contrast to the position of a sunset point, the intersections of the BMP with the horizon at SS and SR are independent of topography. Moreover, partial view of the sky should be sufficient for deducing the SS point from the BMP at sunset or sunrise.

    Figure  1.  Three-dimensional illustration of the band of maximum polarization (BMP) at sunrise and sunset, intersecting the horizon vertically. Course of setting sun in locations with different horizon features, illustrating that the position/timing of the local sunset depends on topography (from Muheim et al., 2007).

    Muheim et al. (2006b, 2007) conducted their cue-conflict experiments in Emlen funnels (Emlen and Emlen, 1966) using wild-caught Savannah Sparrows to test whether these birds recalibrated their magnetic compass relative to the skylight polarization cues. The authors suggested that migrants averaged directional information obtained from skylight polarization patterns at SS and SR to derive a geographic directional system that provides the primary calibration reference for all of the compasses of migratory songbirds (Muheim et al., 2006b). Averaging the directions of SS and SR is an inviting opportunity indeed, because the bisector of the directions towards the SS and SR points is the north-south axis, 365 days a year in any location on the earth. However, the results of Muheim et al. (2006b) do not actually provide evidence for averaging directional information from skylight polarization patterns at SS and SR. The birds shifted their orientation by 90° after being exposed to polarization patterns shifted by 90°, which means that they did not average their orientation, for otherwise the directional shift by the birds should have been 45°, not 90° (as shown by Bill Cochran in a discussion at the RIN Animal Navigation Forum in September 2006).

    Another problem, emphasized by Wiltschko et al. (2008a), is that it is not at all clear whether the Savannah Sparrows, tested by Muheim et al. (2007), were in the migratory disposition in the first place. Neither was the initial distribution of the headings of the birds shown nor was a mean vector given. The sample from which the test birds were selected may not have been oriented towards their migratory direction. The authors selected individuals that oriented to the southeast (the assumed migratory direction) ±90° for further testing. This is a quite problematic procedure, because the birds taken from a sample which is not significantly oriented cannot be assumed to be oriented even if their headings per chance lie in the migratory direction. A second test before exposure to check whether the birds truly headed in their migratory direction was not reported. Therefore, it is not immediately obvious whether the behavior of the birds in this experiment was at all migratory.

    Wiltschko et al. (2008a) obtained contradictory results when testing Australian Silvereyes (Zosterops lateralis) in Emlen funnels. Their birds paid no attention to cue conflict and maintained the same seasonally appropriate migratory direction both before and after exposure. Unlike Muheim et al. (2007) who altered polarization pattern by using polarizers (which is extremely difficult to do in a fashion similar to the natural pattern), Wiltschko et al. (2008a) rotated the magnetic field by Helmholtz coils and let the birds see the naturally polarized sky. Muheim et al. (2008) explained the discrepancy between their results (Muheim et al., 2006b, 2007) and those by Wiltschko et al. (2008a) by the fact that the Australian Silvereyes tested in the latter study could have calibrated migratory compass direction on landmarks they could see in the aviary. However, it is not immediately clear why, if silvereyes in Australia did it as suggested by Muheim et al. (2008), Savannah Sparrows in Alaska studied by Muheim et al. (2006b, 2007) failed to do the same. Summarizing, the claim that Savannah Sparrows calibrated their magnetic compass on skylight polarization cues and, especially that they averaged directions from both SS and SR, seems less than convincing.

    It should be however noted that in later experiments with White-throated Sparrows (Zonotrichia albicollis) Muheim et al. (2009) rotated the magnetic field, similar to Wiltschko et al. (2008a) and obtained the same results (priority of skylight polarization compass) as were recorded for Savannah Sparrows (Muheim et al., 2006b, 2007). Furthermore, White-throated Sparrows tested in spring and in autumn (Muheim et al., 2009) undoubtedly were on migration. Generally, in the latter study the authors addressed some of the concerns voiced by Wiltschko et al. (2008a, 2008b) and their results look more convincing than the results of their earlier work with Savannah Sparrows (Muheim et al., 2006b, 2007).

    Like Wiltschko et al. (2008a), Gaggini et al. (2010) reported that Pied Flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca) did not calibrate their magnetic compass on sunset cues during spring migration in Italy. However, their study is problematic in the same manner as the study by Muheim et al. (2007): the initial sample of untreated birds captured during the spring migration was not significantly oriented in the migratory direction (Fig. 4a in Gaggini et al., 2010). All birds in the Italian study were, beyond doubt, on migration but since they were tested within several days after capture, their behavior may have reflected stress responses rather than migratory direction. Freshly captured migrants, unlike individuals that are given time to get accustomed to captivity, are known often to show phototactic or other stress responses (Marchetti and Baldaccini, 2003). If the birds do not show a meaningful migratory orientation in the first place, all further cue-conflict experiments with them are necessarily problematic.

    In another study performed during autumn migration in Denmark, Pied Flycatchers and Redstarts (Phoenicurus phoenicurus) did not calibrate their magnetic compass from sunset cues (Rabøl, 2010). Their orientation in Emlen funnels during the deep night was not affected by pre-exposure to a horizontally rotated geomagnetic field.

    The study by Cochran et al. (2004) very convincingly showed that the Gray-cheeked Thrush (Catharus minimus) and Swainson's Thrush (C. ustulatus) indeed infer compass direction from sunset cues before take-off and maintain it during nocturnal migratory flight on the basis of their magnetic compass. In this study migrants were not tested in Emlen funnels but were radio-tagged and tracked when flying by natural nocturnal migratory flight. At twilight, these two species of Catharus thrushes were exposed to an artificial magnetic field that was identical to the natural geomagnetic field, but rotated 80° clockwise (Fig. 2). When the birds where released, they immediately found themselves in the natural unaltered magnetic field and flew generally 80° counterclockwise of their normal migratory direction. During the next night, their migratory direction was back to normal, suggesting that compass recalibration occurs every day (Fig. 2). The study by Cochran et al. (2004) is a very convincing one and very clean methodologically. It is difficult to agree with the criticism by Wiltschko et al. (2008a) who claimed that this study is problematic since the birds were free-flying rather than confined to an Emlen funnel. In our opinion, this is a very strong feature of this study and not its drawback. The suggestion that the thrushes of Cochran et al. could be overcompensating after exposure to cue conflict (Wiltschko et al., 2008) does not hold either because, as correctly emphasized by Muheim et al. (2008), these birds showed deflected orientation when tracked immediately upon release just after cue conflict.

    Figure  2.  Predicted orientation responses of birds with respect to potential orientation mechanisms and their interactions. (A) If the stars or the magnetic field show simple domination, or if the stars are calibrated from twilight cues, there should be no effect of the magnetic treatment after release (third and fourth column). (B) If birds use their magnetic compass to calibrate a celestial compass at sunset, they should head east on the first night after treatment and then in their normal northerly migratory direction. (C) If twilight cues are used to calibrate the magnetic compass on a daily basis, birds experiencing a magnetic field turning towards the east, should orient towards the west during the same night after release. On subsequent nights they should return to their normal northerly migratory direction. (A to C) The four thin parallel arrows indicate the horizontal direction of the magnetic field lines experienced by the birds. The thick arrow indicates the expected orientation of the birds. The star indicates the unchanged directional information, potentially available from celestial cues (from Cochran et al., 2004).

    However, a recent study by Chernetsov et al. (2011) aimed to repeat the work of Cochran et al. (2004) in a European medium-distance migrant, the Song Thrush (Turdus philomelos). It was also based on tracking free-flying birds, with the difference that Chernetsov et al. (2011) did not follow the released radio-tagged birds by car, but just recorded their vanishing directions at least 15 km after their nocturnal migratory take-off. Song Thrushes in this study did not calibrate their compass systems, but showed a simple domination of either a magnetic or possibly a stellar compass, as suggested by the lack of any effect of magnetic field horizontal rotation prior to release on their flight directions (Fig. 3).

    Figure  3.  Predicted orientation responses and results of orientation tests performed with song thrushes. (A to C) Predicted responses of magnetically treated birds according to the proposed hypotheses in the fall (the magnetic field deflected 120° clockwise) and spring (the magnetic field deflected 120° anticlockwise), similar to Fig. 2. (D to G) Bearings of song thrushes kept in the ambient magnetic field (spring – D, autumn – F) and in a magnetic field deflected 120° counterclockwise (spring – E) or clockwise (autumn – G) prior to release. D: 26° (n = 7, r = 0.97, p < 0.001); E: 23° (n = 8, r = 0.94, p < 0.001); F: 232° (n = 6, r = 0.95, p < 0.001); G: 249° (n = 5, r = 0.92, p < 0.006). Each dot on the circle periphery represents the bearing of the last point at which the radio signal of the bird was detected. The arrow represents the mean group vector. The inner and outer dashed circles represent the radius of the group mean vector needed for significance (p < 0.05 and p < 0.01, respectively). The two radial dashed lines flanking the group mean vector represent CI (from Chernetsov et al., 2011).

    The studies by Cochran et al. (2004) and Muheim et al. (2006b, 2007, 2009) impressed the avian orientation community greatly and many authors quickly forgot previous results, suggesting the primary role of the magnetic compass (see above). A reason to discard this information was that tracking of free-flying migrants is considered methodologically superior to Emlen funnel tests that, even though broadly used in avian orientation research, still represent a highly unnatural and probably stressful situation for migrants. However, some results suggesting recalibration of celestial cues by magnetic compass were obtained by release experiments, not in Emlen funnels (e.g. Sandberg et al., 2000). Therefore we argue that the very impressive results by Cochran et al. (2004) and Muheim et al. (2006b), providing strong evidence that the two Catharus thrushes and probably Savannah and White-throated Sparrows used skylight polarization pattern as their primary compass cue and regularly recalibrate their magnetic compass on it, should not be immediately generalized to other avian migrants. Even if we believe that the results of Gaggini (2010) suggesting no cue calibration are questionable, we still have the results by Wiltschko et al. (2008a) and Rabøl (2010) obtained by Emlen funnel testing and, rather more importantly, the results from Chernetsov et al. (2011) from free-flying Song Thrushes suggest that some nocturnal migrants do not calibrate compass cues but rely on simple domination.

    It has been suggested by Susanne Åkesson during her talk at the 7th Royal Institute of Navigation 2011 conference "Orientation and Navigation. Birds, Humans and Other Animals" in Reading, UK, that among-species variation observed might be explained by the necessity to cross areas with broadly varying declination values (declination is the angular difference between the magnetic North and the geographic North) in long-distance North American passerine migrants, whereas European migrants and Australian Silvereyes may not face this challenge and remain within the zones with similar inclination values throughout their annual cycle. Another non-excluding possibility mentioned by Chernetsov et al. (2011) is that long-distance migrants generally need a capacity for more precise orientation, whereas short- and medium-distance migrants (e.g. Australian Silvereyes and Song Thrushes) may tolerate larger angular deviations from their inherited compass migratory directions.

  • Aguilar, S., Mugica, L., Acosta, M., Aguilar, K., Boscolo, D., Castro, R., et al., 2021. Effect of landscape structure on waterbirds community in a conservation gradient in southwestern wetlands coast of Cuba. Wetl. Ecol. Manag. 29, 929-944.
    Aharon-Rotman, Y., McEvoy, J., Zheng, Z.J., Yu, H., Wang, X., Ya, L.Y., et al., 2017. Water level affects availability of optimal feeding habitats for threatened migratory waterbirds. Ecol. Evol. 7, 10440-10450.
    An, A., Zhang, Y., Cao, L., Jia, Q., Wang, X., 2018. A potential distribution map of wintering Swan Goose (Anser cygnoides) in the middle and lower Yangtze River floodplain, China. Avian Res. 9, 43.
    An, L., Liao, K., Zhu, L., Zhou, B., 2021. Influence of river-lake isolation on the water level variations of Caizi Lake, lower reach of the Yangtze River. J. Geog. Sci. 31, 551-564.
    Baselga, A., 2010. Partitioning the turnover and nestedness components of beta diversity. Global Ecol. Biogeogr. 19, 134-143.
    Baselga, A., Freckleton, R., 2013. Separating the two components of abundance-based dissimilarity: Balanced changes in abundance vs. abundance gradients. Methods Ecol. Evol. 4, 552-557.
    Basnet, T.B., Rokaya M.B., Bhattarai, B.P., Munzbergova, Z., 2016. Heterogeneous landscapes on steep slopes at low altitudes as hotspots of bird diversity in a hilly region of Nepal in the central Himalayas. PLoS One 11, e0150498.
    Brandolin, P.G., Blendinger, P.G., 2015. Effect of habitat and landscape structure on waterbird abundance in wetlands of central Argentina. Wetl. Ecol. Manag. 24, 93-105.
    Cao, L., Fox, A.D., 2009. Birds and people both depend on China's wetlands. Nature 460, 173.
    Chase, J.M., Leibold, M.A., 2002. Spatial scale dictates the productivity-biodiversity relationship. Nature 416, 427-430.
    Chen, C.W., Holyoak, M., Wang, Y.P., Si, X.F., Ding, P., 2019. Spatiotemporal distribution of seasonal bird assemblages on land-bridge islands: Linking dynamic and static views of metacommunities. Avian Res. 10, 25.
    Chen, J.Y., Zhou, L.Z., Zhou, B., Xu, R.X., Zhu, W.Z., Xu, W.B., 2011. Seasonal dynamics of wintering waterbirds in two shallow lakes along Yangtze River in Anhui Province. Zool. Res. 32, 540-548.
    Cumming, G.S., Paxton, M., King, J., Beuster, H., 2012. Foraging guild membership explains variation in waterbird responses to the hydrological regime of an arid-region flood-pulse river in Namibia. Freshw. Biol. 57, 1202-1213.
    de Arruda Almeida, B., Sebastián-González, E., dos Anjos, L., Green, A.J., Botella, F., 2019. A functional perspective for breeding and wintering waterbird communities: Temporal trends in species and trait diversity. Oikos 128, 1103-1115.
    Fan, Y.G., Zhou, L.Z., Cheng, L., Song, Y.W., Xu, W.B., 2020. Foraging behavior of the Greater White-fronted Goose (Anser albifrons) wintering at shengjin lake: diet shifts and habitat use. Avian Res. 11, 3.
    Faragó, S., Hangya, K., 2012. Effects of water level on waterbird abundance and diversity along the middle section of the Danube River. Hydrobiologia 697, 15-21.
    Fleming, K.S., Kaminski, R.M., Schummer, M.L., Nelms, K.D., Ervin, G.N., Tietjen, T.E., 2015. Species richness and density of wintering ducks on wetlands reserve program easements in Mississippi. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 39, 310-318.
    Ganzhorn, J.U., Eisenbeiß, B., 2001. The the concept of nested species assemblages and its utility for understanding effects of habitat fragmentation. Basic Appl. Ecol. 2, 87-99.
    Gemmer, M., Jiang, T., Su, B., Kundzewicz, Z.W., 2008. Seasonal precipitation changes in the wet season and their influence on flood/drought hazards in the Yangtze River Basin, China. Quat. Int. 186, 12-21.
    Götzenberger, L., de Bello, F., Bråthen, K.A., Davison, J., Dubuis, A., Guisan, A., et al., 2012. Ecological assembly rules in plant communities-approaches, patterns and prospects. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 87, 111-127.
    Guadagnin, D.L., Maltchik, L., Fonseca, C.R., 2009. Species-area relationship of Neotropical waterbird assemblages in remnant wetlands: Looking at the mechanisms. Divers. Distrib. 15, 319-327.
    Henry, D.A.W., Cumming, G.S., 2016. Spatial and environmental processes show temporal variation in the structuring of waterbird metacommunities. Ecosphere. 7, e01451.
    Huang, Y., Chen, X.S., Zou, Y.A., Zhang, P.Y., Li, F., Hou, Z.Y., et al., 2022. Exploring the relative contribution of flood regimes and climatic factors to Carex phenology in a Yangtze River-connected floodplain wetland. Sci. Total Environ. 847, 157568.
    Jia, Q., Wang, X., Zhang, Y., Cao, L., Fox, A.D., 2018. Drivers of waterbird communities and their declines on Yangtze River floodplain lakes. Biol. Conserv. 218, 240-246.
    Klein, E., Berg, E.E., Dial, R., 2005. Wetland drying and succession across the Kenai Peninsula Lowlands, south-central Alaska. Can. J. For. Res. 35, 1931-1941.
    Lee, E., Sagong, J., Lee, Y., 2020. Influence of land use change on the waterbird community of Sihwa Lake, Republic of Korea. Avian Res. 11, 36.
    Legendre, P., 2014. Interpreting the replacement and richness difference components of beta diversity. Global Ecol. Biogeogr. 23, 1324-1334.
    Legendre, P., De Caceres, M., 2013. Beta diversity as the variance of community data: Dissimilarity coefficients and partitioning. Ecol. Lett. 16, 951-963.
    Leibold, M.A., Holyoak, M., Mouquet, N., Amarasekare, P., Chase, J.M., Hoopes, M.F., et al., 2004. The metacommunity concept: A framework for multi-scale community ecology. Ecol. Lett. 7, 601-613.
    Li, C.L., Li, H.F., Zhang, Y., Zha, D.D., Zhao, B.B., Yang, S., et al., 2019a. Predicting hydrological impacts of the Yangtze-to-Huaihe Water Diversion Project on habitat availability for wintering waterbirds at Caizi Lake. J. Environ. Manage. 249, 109251.
    Li, C.L., Yang, Y., Wang, Z., Yang, L., Zhang, D.M., Zhou, L.Z., 2018. The relationship between seasonal water level fluctuation and habitat availability for wintering waterbirds at Shengjin Lake, China. Bird. Conserv. Int. 29, 100-114.
    Li, C.L., Zhang, Y., Zha, D.D., Yang, S., Huang, Z.Y.X., Boer, W.F., et al., 2019b. Assembly processes of waterbird communities across subsidence wetlands in China: A functional and phylogenetic approach. Divers. Distrib. 25, 1118-1129.
    Li, Y.K., Qian, F.W., Silbernagel, J., Larson, H., 2019c. Community structure, abundance variation and population trends of waterbirds in relation to water level fluctuation in Poyang Lake. J. Great Lake. Res. 45, 976-985.
    Li, Y.K., Zhong, Y.F., Shao, R.Q., Yan, C., Jin, J.F., Shan, J.H., et al., 2020. Modified hydrological regime from the Three Gorges Dam increases the risk of food shortages for wintering waterbirds in Poyang Lake. Global Ecol. Conserv. 24, e01286.
    Liu, X.J., Min, F.Y., Kettner, A.J., 2019. The impact of large to extreme flood events on floodplain evolution of the middle and lower reaches of the Yangtze River, China. Catena 176, 394-409.
    Mei, X., Dai, Z., Du, J., Chen, J., 2015. Linkage between Three Gorges Dam impacts and the dramatic recessions in China's largest freshwater lake, Poyang Lake. Sci. Rep. 5, 18197.
    Meynard, C.N., Devictor, V., Mouillot, D., Thuiller, W., Jiguet, F., Mouquet N., 2011. Beyond taxonomic diversity patterns: how do α, β and γ components of bird functional and phylogenetic diversity respond to environmental gradients across France? Global Ecol. Biogeogr. 20, 893-903.
    Ndehedehe, C.E., Onojeghuo, A.O., Stewart-Koster, B., Bunn, S.E., Ferreira, V.G., 2021. Upstream flows drive the productivity of floodplain ecosystems in tropical Queensland. Ecol. Indicat. 125.
    Palmer, M.A., Menninger, H.L., Bernhardt, E., Ormerod, S.J., Dobson, M., Hildrew, A.G., et al., 2010. River restoration, habitat heterogeneity and biodiversity: A failure of theory or practice? Freshw. Biol. 55, 205-222.
    Qian, H., Ricklefs, R.E., White, P.S., 2005. Beta diversity of angiosperms in temperate floras of eastern Asia and eastern North America. Ecol. Lett. 8, 15-22.
    Roswell, M., Dushoff, J., Winfree, R., 2021. A conceptual guide to measuring species diversity. Oikos 130, 321-338.
    Schuh, M.H., Guadagnin, D.L., 2018. Habitat and landscape factors associated with the nestedness of waterbird assemblages and wetland habitats in South Brazil. Austral. Ecol. 43, 989-999.
    Wang, W.J., Wang, Y.F., Hou, J.J., Ouyang, S., 2019. Flooding influences waterbird abundance at Poyang Lake, China. Waterbirds 42, 30-38.
    Wang, X., Zhu, G., Ma, H.H., Wu, Y., Zhang, W.W., Zhang, Y., et al., 2022. Bird communities’ responses to human-modified landscapes in the southern Anhui Mountainous Area. Avian Res. 13, 100006.
    Wang, X.J., Chen, J.Y., Zhou, L.Z., 2020. Effects of human activities on the diversity of waterbirds wintering in a shallow lake of the middle and lower Yangtze River floodplain, China. Diversity 12, 302.
    Wei, Z.H., Zheng, M., Zhou, L.Z., Xu, W.B., 2020. Flexible foraging response of wintering Hooded Cranes (Grus monacha) to food availability in the lakes of the Yangtze River floodplain, China. Animals 10, 568.
    Whittaker, R.H., 1960. Vegetation of the Siskiyou Mountains, Oregon and California. Ecol. Monogr. 30, 279-338.
    Wilson, D.S., 1992. Complex interactions in metacommunities, with implications for biodiversity and higher levels of selection. Ecology 73, 1984-2000.
    Wright, D.H., Reeves, J.H., 1992. On the meaning and measurement of nestedness of species assemblages. Oecologia 92, 416-428.
    Xia, S.X., Liu, Y., Chen, B., Jia, Y.F., Zhang, H., Liu, G.H., et al., 2017. Effect of water level fluctuations on wintering goose abundance in Poyang Lake wetlands of China. Chin. Geogr. Sci. 27, 248-258.
    Xia, S.X., Liu, Y., Wang, Y.Y., Chen, B., Jia, Y.F., Liu, G.H., et al., 2016. Wintering waterbirds in a large river floodplain: Hydrological connectivity is the key for reconciling development and conservation. Sci. Total Environ. 573, 645-660.
    Xia, S.X., Yu, X.B., Lei, J.Y., Hearn, R., Smith, B., Lei, G., et al., 2020. Priority sites and conservation gaps of wintering waterbirds in the Yangtze River floodplain. J. Geog. Sci. 30, 1617-1632.
    Yang, S.L., 2005. Impact of dams on Yangtze River sediment supply to the sea and delta intertidal wetland response. J. Geophys. Res. 110.
    Yang, W., Li, X.X., Sun, T., Yang, Z.F., Li, M., 2017. Habitat heterogeneity affects the efficacy of ecological restoration by freshwater releases in a recovering freshwater coastal wetland in China’s Yellow River Delta. Ecol. Eng. 104, 1-12.
    Yu, C., Zhou, L.Z., Mahtab, N., Fan, S.J., Song, Y.W., 2019. The Influence of food density, flock size, and disturbance on the functional response of Bewick's swans (Cygnus columbianus bewickii) in wintering habitats. Animals 9, 946.
    Zhao, Y.H., Sanders, N.J., Liu, J., Jin, T.H., Zhou, H.N., Lu, R.S., et al., 2021. β diversity among ant communities on fragmented habitat islands: the roles of species trait, phylogeny and abundance. Ecography 44, 1568-1578.
    Zhou, J., Zhou, L.Z., Xu, W.B., 2020. Diversity of wintering waterbirds enhanced by restoring aquatic vegetation at Shengjin Lake, China. Sci. Total Environ. 737, 140190.
  • Related Articles

Catalog

    Figures(3)  /  Tables(2)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (31) PDF downloads (22) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return