Jin-Ming Zhao, Li-Zhi Zhou. 2018: Area, isolation, disturbance and age effects on species richness of summer waterbirds in post-mining subsidence lakes, Anhui, China. Avian Research, 9(1): 8. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-018-0101-1
Citation: Jin-Ming Zhao, Li-Zhi Zhou. 2018: Area, isolation, disturbance and age effects on species richness of summer waterbirds in post-mining subsidence lakes, Anhui, China. Avian Research, 9(1): 8. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-018-0101-1

Area, isolation, disturbance and age effects on species richness of summer waterbirds in post-mining subsidence lakes, Anhui, China

Funds: 

the National Natural Science Foundation of China 31472020

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Zhou Li-Zhi, zhoulz@ahu.edu.cn; zhoulz2009@qq.com

  • Received Date: 28 Sep 2017
  • Accepted Date: 06 Mar 2018
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 08 Mar 2018
  • Background 

    The species–area relationship is one of the best documented patterns in community ecology. Factors influencing species–area relationship are still not well understood. Since the proposal of the equilibrium theory of island biogeography, many studies have tested the effects of area and isolation, estimating effects of disturbance and patch age were scarce and should also be important bearing.

    Methods 

    We investigated summer waterbird species richness in 38 post-mining subsidence lakes in Huainan and Fuyang, Anhui, China, May 25–June 12, 2016. We examined the effects of lake area, isolation (distance to natural water bodies), aquaculture disturbance, lake age and lake state (still sinking or not sinking) on summer waterbird species richness with multivariable linear regression method and model selection method.

    Results 

    Our results from multivariable linear regression and model selection were consistent, which showed that species richness was positively related with lake area and negatively related with isolation. Aquaculture disturbance negatively influenced species richness. Highest species richness were found in lakes under intermediate intensity of aquaculture activities with area less than 100 ha and lakes under low intensity of aquaculture activities with area greater than 100 ha. Lake age and lake state had no significant effects on species richness.

    Conclusions 

    Species richness was most related with lake area, aquaculture disturbance, and isolation, with lake area had a positive effect, while aquaculture activities and isolation had a negative effect. What's more, aquaculture activities significantly changed the slope of species–area relationship and might impede us finding a relationship between species richness and lake age.

  • Laying sequence in birds strongly affects the color, mass, metabolic rate, and embryonic development rate of the eggs (Ruxton et al. 2001; Moreno and Osorno 2003; Griffith and Gilby 2013). The pigments covering eggshells are deposited in the 4 h preceding egg laying by pigment glands (Burley and Vadehra 1989). During the laying period, the pigments in some birds gradually decrease as the number of eggs laid increases, which results in a shift in eggshell color from dark to light over the course of the laying sequence. This phenomenon, which is relatively common among the Passer genus (Yom-Tov 1980; Ruxton et al. 2001; Poláček et al. 2017), produces a clutch with a diversity of egg colors; the lightest egg is the last one laid (Lowther 1988; Poláček et al. 2017). Previous studies showed that variation in egg color reflected differences in egg quality; the last eggs to be laid with lighter coloration were the poorest quality (Moreno and Osorno 2003; Soler et al. 2005). Parent birds often distribute resources, such as antioxidants and male hormones, unevenly over the course of the laying sequence (Hall et al. 2010; Boncoraglio et al. 2011). Therefore, eggs laid at different points in the sequence receive different resources, which could influence their embryonic development and ultimate fate (Lipar and Ketterson 2000; Nicolai et al. 2004; Groothuis et al. 2005; Granroth-Wilding et al. 2014; Poláček et al. 2017; Cao et al. 2018). Some female use big size and other maternal resources by last laid eggs to compensate for the survival disadvantage experienced (Saino et al. 2010; Newbrey et al. 2015).

    Magrath et al. (2009) showed that, given consistent environmental conditions, eggs laid earlier start to develop before the female bird has finished laying the entire clutch, so the first eggs laid hatch before the last eggs do. As a result, the clutch includes nestlings with an extreme imbalance in mass quality (Magrath et al. 2009). However, other research indicated that the eggs of Canada Geese (Branta canadensis) from the same clutch hatched at about the same time, with a maximum time interval of less than 24 h (Boonstra et al. 2010). Therefore, the embryonic development rate differed for eggs within the same clutch.

    So far only a small number of studies have focused on the relationship between laying sequence and embryonic internal development rate in birds showing that laying sequence can affect embryonic development (Nicolai et al. 2004; Boonstra et al. 2010; Cao et al. 2018; Yang et al. 2018). Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia Guttata) clutches number can alter the developmental time of embryos, egg maternal resource allocation across the laying sequence can influence development (Griffith and Gilby 2013). In this study, using artificial nest boxes, we compared the effects of laying sequence on eggshell color variation and embryonic development speed of Russet Sparrows (Passer cinnamomeus), a passerine species.

    This study was conducted in the Kuankuoshui National Nature Reserve (28°10ʹN, 107°10ʹE), a subtropical evergreen broadleaf forest in southwestern China at an altitude of about 1500 m (see Yang et al. 2010 for more details) from April to August 2013.

    The Russet Sparrow belongs to the Passeridae family and is mainly found in southern China and the Himalayan region (Summers-Smith 1988). They are year-round residents at this study site, and they build nests in chimney tubes, cavities of stone walls and eaves (Yang et al. 2012b; Huo et al. 2018). Artificial nest boxes were provided to attract sparrows because entrances to their natural nests are usually too deep and small to allow easy access (see below).

    Nest boxes were 35 cm in depth with an entrance hole diameter of 4 cm (Fig. 1). We checked each box periodically until the first Russet Sparrow egg was observed in the nest, after which the nests were checked once a day to confirm if the onset of incubation start with the first egg or until after clutch completion. When a new egg was discovered in the nest, the tip of the egg was numbered with a light blue marker to indicate egg-laying order. After a full clutch of eggs had been laid, they were all replaced with model eggs and moved to an incubator together. We did so because Russet Sparrows start their incubation after clutch completion and accept any model eggs in their nests and seem not to show any egg discrimination (Huo et al. 2018). After the nestlings hatched, they were returned to their original nests to be raised by their parents.

    Figure  1.  The nest box used and a male Russet Sparrow. Photo by Bruce Lyon

    We divided the sparrow eggs into three groups. The first eggs to be laid were first group (one egg), the eggs that were intermediate in the laying sequence were middle group (two or three eggs), and the last eggs to be laid were last group (one egg). The middle group has two or three eggs, so we analyze the data using an average for the middle eggs. A caliper (505-681, Mitutoyo, Japan; range 0-150 mm, accuracy ± 0.02 mm) was used to measure the length and width of the eggs. An electronic balance (EHA501, Guangdong Xiangshan Weighing Apparatus Group Co., Ltd.; range 0-100 g; accuracy 0.01 g) was used to weigh the eggs. The volume of the eggs was calculated according to Hoyt (1979): egg volume = 0.51 × egg length × square egg width. The eggs in each group were weighed from the first day of incubation, and daily afterward, until nestlings had hatched. Egg color was quantified using a USB4000 VIS-NIR spectrometer (Ocean Optics, Inc., Dunedin, Florida, USA). Background and spot color were measured at three randomly chosen places on each egg. A detailed description of the method can be found in Yang et al.(2009, 2010). The values of the ultra-violet (300-400 nm) and visible (400-700 nm) sections of the reflectance spectrum were used for analysis. The eggs were hatched in a Mini EX digital incubator (Brinsea Co. Ltd., UK; also see Yang et al. 2018).

    Embryonic heart rates were measured using a digital egg monitor MK1 type heart rate meter (Buddy Digital Egg Monitor, Avitronics Inc., Cornwall, England). In order to avoid errors in heart rate measurement due to differences in temperature, the eggs were placed horizontally in the heart rate meter for the measurement immediately after they were removed from the incubator (also see Zhao et al. 2017; Cao et al. 2018; Yang et al. 2018). The heart rate (beats per minute) was recorded after the heart rate became stable. Heart rate measurements started on the first day of incubation and were conducted every other day, until the nestlings hatched. The embryonic development time was defined as the time interval between the first appearance of heart rate (which occurred 2-5 days after the start of incubation) and hatching (Cao et al. 2018).

    The incubator hatching temperature was set at 7.5 ± 0.5 ℃; the relative humidity was 55 ± 5%; the egg turning interval was r/45 min; the cooling time was 2 h/day. One-sample Kolmogorov-Smimov (K-S) test was used to analyze the normality distribution of the data. If the data satisfied the conditions of normal distribution, one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare the means. If the data were not normally distributed, Welch's ANOVA was used. Differences at p < 0.05 were considered statistically significant and at p < 0.01 were highly statistically significant. Unless otherwise stated, data were presented as mean ± SD, and all tests were 2-tailed. Statistical analyses were performed in IBM SPSS 20.0 for Windows (IBM Inc., USA).

    Each Russet Sparrow clutch contained 3-5 eggs (n = 69). One egg was laid each day, normally in the early morning. Whitening in last eggs was relatively apparent to the naked eye in the majority of nests because pale colors reflect more ambient light than dark colors (78.5%, n = 69) (Fig. 2). Background color reflectance of last eggs was significantly higher than those in the other two groups, confirming that whitening occurred (Fig. 3). Spot color reflectance among the three groups was similar, all spots being dark brown (Fig. 3). The brightness reflectance of the background color was significantly higher in last eggs than in the other groups (F = 10.817, p = 0.001). Except for green light chromaticity, the other lights' chromaticity of the background color showed significant differences between the groups. Last eggs had the highest ultraviolet (UV) and blue light chromaticity (F = 3.531, p = 0.041; F = 3.986, p = 0.028) and the lowest red light and yellow light chromaticity (F = 3.890, p = 0.030; F = 3.655, p = 0.037). However, there was no difference in background color hue among the three groups (F = 1.518, p = 0.234) (Table 1). The spots on the eggs in the three groups did not significantly differ in brightness, hue, or chromaticity (Table 2).

    Figure  2.  Whitening of the last eggs to be laid in Russet Sparrow clutches. a, b, and c are three clutches of Russet Sparrow eggs, respectively, placed from left to right according to their laying orders. Photo by Juan Huo
    Figure  3.  Background reflectance of Russet Sparrow eggs. G1 refers to first group, the first eggs to be laid, G2 to middle group, the intermediate eggs in the sequence, and G3 to last group, the last eggs in each clutch. Back means eggshell background color, spot means eggshell spot color
    Table  1.  Background reflectance parameters of Russet Sparrow eggs
    First group (n = 12) Middle group (n = 12) Last group (n = 12) F Sig.
    Brightness 13, 211.97 ± 4730.26 13, 668.24 ± 3956.62 20, 162.99 ± 3504.98 10.817 0.001
    Hue 0.827 ± 0.712 0.901 ± 0.321 0.378 ± 1.136 1.518 0.234
    UV-chroma 0.202 ± 0.030 0.212 ± 0.029 0.231 ± 0.024 3.531 0.041
    B-chroma 0.172 ± 0.016 0.168 ± 0.014 0.183 ± 0.008 3.986 0.028
    G-chroma 0.198 ± 0.009 0.193 ± 0.006 0.194 ± 0.007 1.450 0.249
    Y-chroma 0.209 ± 0.017 0.208 ± 0.016 0.194 ± 0.011 3.890 0.030
    R-chroma 0.219 ± 0.027 0.219 ± 0.024 0.198 ± 0.014 3.655 0.037
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV
    Table  2.  Spot reflectance parameters of Russet Sparrow eggs
    First group (n = 12) Middle group (n = 12) Last group (n = 12) F Sig.
    Brightness 2952.68 ± 4730.26 3413.34 ± 1126.44 3532.50 ± 1515.39 0.632 0.538
    Hue 0.373 ± 0.358 0.522 ± 0.287 0.464 ± 0.177 0.835 0.443
    UV-chroma 0.247 ± 0.046 0.236 ± 0.048 0.244 ± 0.037 0.215 0.808
    B-chroma 0.149 ± 0.020 0.143 ± 0.019 0.144 ± 0.020 0.245 0.784
    G-chroma 0.159 ± 0.014 0.162 ± 0.012 0.155 ± 0.012 1.001 0.378
    Y-chroma 0.198 ± 0.025 0.207 ± 0.025 0.201 ± 0.020 0.397 0.675
    R-chroma 0.247 ± 0.039 0.252 ± 0.040 0.256 ± 0.043 0.126 0.882
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    When the first Russet Sparrow egg was observed in the nest, we checked once a day to confirm that Russet Sparrows start their incubation until after clutch completion. The three groups did not differ in egg size, mass, or weight loss during development (Table 3). Hatching success rates of first, middle and last groups were 41.7, 46.1, and 25.0%, respectively, without significant differences (χ2 = 1.55, p = 0.46).

    Table  3.  Sizes of Russet Sparrow eggs produced at different points in the laying sequence
    First group (n = 12) Middle group (n = 12) Last group (n = 12) F Sig.
    Mass (g) 2.020 ± 0.178 2.025 ± 0.129 2.028 ± 0.172 0.008 0.992
    Length (mm) 19.128 ± 0.532 18.975 ± 0.489 19.071 ± 0.566 0.256 0.775
    Width (mm) 14.117 ± 0.478 14.118 ± 0.413 14.165 ± 0.604 0.035 0.965
    Volume (cm3) 1.947 ± 0.152 1.930 ± 0.121 1.944 ± 0.183 0.083 0.921
    Mass loss (g) 0.184 ± 0.023 0.185 ± 0.040 0.210 ± 0.034 0.656 0.533
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    The embryonic heart rates among the three groups of eggs followed approximately the same trend. Embryonic heart rate increased as the number of incubation days increased (Fig. 4). Heart rates became detectable between two and five days after the start of incubation but appeared latest in last eggs. The highest heart rates appeared on the day of hatching (i.e., day 13) for first and middle eggs, but on day 11 in last eggs. Regression analysis using linear and cubic models showed that a regression curve was the best fit for the heart rate change in first eggs, indicating that the heart rates of these eggs were the most stable. The regression curve fit was the poorest in last eggs, indicating that embryonic heart rate change in these eggs fluctuated the most. The incubation period was around 13 days in all three groups, without significant differences (F = 0.589, p = 0.566). The embryonic development time of the last eggs was significantly shorter than those of the first eggs (F = 3.60, p = 0.040) (Fig. 5).

    Figure  4.  Embryonic heart rates of Russet Sparrow. Linear and cubic models were used in regression analysis showing that a regression curve was the best fit for the heart rate change in the first egg group and the poorest in the last egg group
    Figure  5.  Incubation period and development time of Russet Sparrow. Development time was a subset of incubation period and was the interval between the first detection of a heart beat and hatching. The lower case letters a and b refer to the results of the LSD (Least Significant Difference) test. The difference between first and last groups was highly significant

    Our spectroscopic analysis showed that Russet Sparrow eggs gradually became lighter as the laying sequence progressed, with a relatively large eggshell color variation coefficient. This change is mainly manifested in the background color of the eggs. Lowther (1988) believed that the amount of pigment secreted by glands in birds is limited, and the pigments gradually subside as egg-laying progresses. Therefore, pigments could be exhausted or in short supply by the time the last egg is laid, which results in its whitened appearance. However, in this study, whitening also appeared in eggs that were laid earlier. Hence, Lowther's hypothesis does not hold here.

    Some researchers believe that egg colors of cave-nesting birds tend to be white and to lack spots. One reason might be that white eggs are more easily seen by parent birds in cave nests with lowlight (Lack 1958).

    Another selective pressure, the existence of nest parasites, can force hosts to increase the evenness of egg color in a clutch, as it enable parent birds to more easily detect an intruder's egg and at the same time increase the diversity of egg color among clutches of the same species (Stokke et al. 2002; Spottiswoode and Stevens 2011). One hypothesis states that the whitening of the last eggs to be laid could be a signal that prevents nest parasites, by informing the parasites that the hosts have completed egg-laying and the egg incubation period has started (Yom-Tov 1980; Ruxton et al. 2001). Although there are many parasitic cuckoo species in the study area (Yang et al. 2011, 2012a), despite evidence that cuckoos can parasitize cave-nesting birds and that some cave-nesting birds are suitable hosts for cuckoos (Deng 2013; Grim et al. 2014; Liang et al. 2016), Russet Sparrow was not the host for any type of cuckoos (Huo et al. 2018) in this study area and no intra-species parasitism has been observed so far. Hence, the evolution of egg color diversity of Russet Sparrow needs further investigation in the future.

    Previous studies believed that the color of birds' eggs is related to the mass of eggs and that different egg colors reflect different levels of egg quality (Moreno and Osorno 2003; Morales et al. 2006; Newbrey et al. 2015). Our data showed that the color of Russet Sparrow eggs was not associated with egg size or mass, and laying order also did not affect size or mass. However, egg-laying order did affect embryonic development rate. Russet Sparrow eggs in the same clutch had the same hatching period and all hatched on the same day (within 24 h). The heart rates of eggs laid earlier were detected earlier, and these eggs had relatively longer embryonic development periods, while the heart rates of eggs laid later were detected later. These eggs had shorter embryonic development periods but higher development speed. In House Sparrows (Passer domesticus), most eggs of a clutch hatched synchronously and there was no difference in hatchability in relation to sequence of egg-laying (Singer and Yom-Tov 1988). Full incubation began after the completion of the clutch in clutches of two and three eggs in both House Sparrows and clutches of four eggs in Tree Sparrows (P. montanus) but before the clutch was completed (by up to one day) in larger clutches (Seel 1968). In Black Brant (Branta bernicla nigricans), embryonic metabolic rate may affect development in synchronizing hatch (Nicolai et al. 2004). In addition, the decline in egg size as laying progresses could contribute to hatching synchrony because those subsequent embryos tend to have higher metabolic rates which hypothesized also contributes to relatively more rapid development (Nicolai et al. 2004). Such phenomenon has also been verified in Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia guttata), a passerine bird, as well as in some non-passerines (Nicolai et al. 2004; Boonstra et al. 2010; Griffith and Gilby 2013; Granroth-Wilding et al. 2014; Hemmings and Birkhead 2016). The number of Zebra Finch eggs in a clutch also influences the eggs' embryonic development speed (Griffith and Gilby 2013).

    Hatching asynchrony results in a brood with nestlings that differ in age and size. Older and larger nestlings usually outcompete younger ones for food and, as a result, grow more quickly (Zach 1982; Kacelnik et al. 1995), while the last-hatched nestling reduced growth and survival (Wiebe 1996; Ostreiher 1997). Complete hatching synchrony leads to uniformly aged nestlings (Clark and Wilson 1981; Stoleson and Beissinger 1995). The hatching time of some birds can be affected by the vocalizations of parent birds or nestlings of the same clutch, as these sounds can stimulate the embryo to speed its development (Saino and Fasola 1996).

    In summary, we showed that the background color brightness of the last eggs laid by Russet Sparrows was significantly higher (whiter) than those of the other eggs. However, all eggs were hatched around 13 days, indicating that laying sequence significantly affected embryonic development speed; the last eggs to be laid developed significantly faster than did the first in the clutch. Our study also indicated that laying sequence has an important effect on the embryonic heart rate in Russet Sparrows.

    WL designed the experiment, JH, TS and NN conducted the experiments and collected field data. JH and CY performed data analyses and wrote an earlier version of the manuscript. WL revised and improved the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

    We would like to thank Prof. Anders Pape Møller for his helpful comments on this manuscript. We thank Junqiu Wu, Guoxian Liang, and Ping Ye for their assistance with fieldwork, and the help and cooperation from the Kuankuoshui National Nature Reserves.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

    Not applicable.

    The experiments comply with the current laws of China, where they were performed. Permit for this study was approved by Kuankuoshui National Nature Reserves, Guizhou Province. Experimental procedures were in agreement with the Animal Research Ethics Committee of Hainan Provincial Education Centre for Ecology and Environment, Hainan Normal University (permit nos. HNECEE-2011-002 and HNECEE-2013-001).

  • Amato KR, Yeoman CJ, Kent A, Righini N, Carbonero F, Estrada A, Gaskins HR, Stumpf RM, Yildirim S, Torralba M. Habitat degradation impacts black howler monkey (Alouatta pigra) gastrointestinal microbiomes. ISME J. 2013;7:1344–53.
    Akaike H. Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. In: Petrov BN, Csaki F, editors. Proceeding of the 2nd international symposium on information theory. Budapest: Akademiai Kiado; 1973. p. 267–81.
    Badano EI, Regidor HA, Núñez HA, Acosta R, Gianoli E. Species richness and structure of ant communities in a dynamic archipelago: effects of island area and age. J Biogeogr. 2005;32:221–7.
    Brown VK, Southwood TRE. Trophic diversity, niche breadth and generation times of exopterygote insects in a secondary succession. Oecologia. 1987;56:220–5.
    Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. Berlin: Springer; 2002.
    Bush MB, Whittaker RJ. Krakatau: colonization patterns and hierarchies. J Biogeogr. 1991;18:341–56.
    Connel JH. Diversity in tropical rain forests and coral reefs—high diversity of trees and corals is maintained only in a non-equilibrium state. Science. 1978;199:1302–10.
    Connor EF, McCoy ED. The statistics and biology of the species–area relationship. Am Nat. 1979;113:791–833.
    Crowe TM. Lots of weeds: insular phytogeography of vacant urban lots. J Biogeogr. 1979;6:169–81.
    Durrett R, Levin S. Spatial models for species–area curves. J Theor Biol. 1996;179:119–27.
    Economo EP, Janda M, Guénard B, Sarnat E. Assembling a species–area curve through colonization, speciation and human-mediated introduction. J Biogeogr. 2016;44:1088–97.
    Feaga JS. Winter waterbird use and food resources of aquaculture lands in Mississippi. Mississippi State: Master of Science, Mississippi State University; 2014.
    Fox BJ, Fox MD. Factors determining mammal species richness on habitat islands and isolates: habitat diversity, disturbance, species interactions and guild assembly rules. Glob Ecol Biogeogr. 2000;9:19–37.
    Frey J, Bogan M, Yates T. Mountaintop island age determines species richness of boreal mammals in the American Southwest. Ecography. 2007;30:231–40.
    Graham MH. Confronting multicollinearity in ecological multiple regression. Ecology. 2003;84:2809–15.
    Hiddink JG, Jennings S, Kaiser MJ, Queirós AM, Duplisea DE, Piet GJ. Cumulative impacts of seabed trawl disturbance on benthic biomass, production, and species richness in different habitats. Can J Fish Aquat Sci. 2006;63:721–36.
    Horsák M, Hájek M, Spitale D, Hájková P, Dítě D, Nekola JC. The age of island-like habitats impacts habitat specialist species richness. Ecology. 2012;93:1106–14.
    Hosmer D, Lemeshow S. Applied logistic regression. 2nd ed. New York: Wiley; 2000.
    Hubbell SP. A unified theory of biogeography and relative species abundance and its application to tropical rain forests and coral reefs. Coral Reefs. 1997;16:S9–21.
    Krauss J, Alfert T, Steffan-Dewenter I. Habitat area but not habitat age determines wild bee richness in limestone quarries. J Appl Ecol. 2009;46:194–202.
    Kreft H, Jetz W, Mutke J, Kier G, Barthlott W. Global diversity of island floras from a macroecological perspective. Ecol Lett. 2008;11:116–27.
    Lantz SM, Gawlik DE, Cook MI. The effects of water depth and submerged aquatic vegetation on the selection of foraging habitat and foraging success of wading birds. Condor. 2010;112:460–9.
    Lomolino MV. The species–area relationship: new challenges for an old pattern. Prog Phys Geogr. 2001;25:1–21.
    Ma Z, Cai Y, Li B, Chen J. Managing wetland habitats for waterbirds: an international perspective. Wetlands. 2010;30:15–27.
    MacArthur RH, Wilson EO. An equilibrium theory of insular zoogeography. Evolution. 1963;17:373–87.
    MacArthur RH, Wilson EO. The theory of island biogeography. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1967.
    Odum EP. The strategy of ecosystem development. Science. 1969;164:262.
    Palmer MW, White PS. Scale dependence and the species–area relationship. Am Nat. 1994;144:717–40.
    Power DM. Numbers of bird species on the California islands. Evolution. 1972;26:451–63.
    Rejmánek M, Rejmánková E. Biogeography of artificial islands: effects of age, area, elevation, and isolation on plant species richness. Preslia. 2002;74:307–14.
    Rosenzweig ML. Species diversity in space and time. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1995.
    Ross KA, Fox BJ, Fox MD. Changes to plant species richness in forest fragments: fragment age, disturbance and fire history may be as important as area. J Biogeogr. 2002;29:749–65.
    Rybicki J, Hanski I. Species–area relationships and extinctions caused by habitat loss and fragmentation. Ecol Lett. 2013;16:27–38.
    Scheffer M, Van Geest G, Zimmer K, Jeppesen E, Søndergaard M, Butler M, Hanson M, Declerck S, De Meester L. Small habitat size and isolation can promote species richness: second-order effects on biodiversity in shallow lakes and ponds. Oikos. 2006;112:227–31.
    Scheiner SM. Six types of species–area curves. Glob Ecol Biogeogr. 2003;12:441–7.
    Simberloff D. Species turnover and equilibrium island biogeography. Science. 1976;194:572.
    Sousa WP. The role of disturbance in natural communities. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1984;15:353–91.
    Storch D, Izling AL, Gaston KJ. Geometry of the species–area relationship in central European birds: testing the mechanism. J Anim Ecol. 2003;72:509–19.
    Storch D, Keil P, Jetz W. Universal species–area and endemics–area relationships at continental scales. Nature. 2012;488:78–81.
    Tittensor DP, Micheli F, Nyström M, Worm B. Human impacts on the specie-area relationship in reef fish assemblages. Ecol Lett. 2007;10:760–72.
    Tischendorf L, Fahrig L. On the usage and measurement of landscape connectivity. Oikos. 2000;90:7–19.
    Tischendorf L, Fahrig L. On the use of connectivity measures in spatial ecology. A reply. Oikos. 2001;95:152–5.
    Valente LM, Etienne RS, Phillimore AB. The effects of island ontogeny on species diversity and phylogeny. Proc Biol Sci. 2014;281:20133227.
    Vera CF, Rocha CF. Habitat disturbance and small mammal richness and diversity in an Atlantic rainforest area in southeastern Brazil. Brazil J Biol. 2006;66:983–90.
    Wagner CE, Harmon LJ, Seehausen O. Cichlid species–area relationships are shaped by adaptive radiations that scale with area. Ecol Lett. 2014;17:583–92.
    Wan WJ, Zhou LZ, Ping L. Impact of coal mining subsidence on species composition and diversity of avian community. Sichuan J Zool. 2015;34:773–9 (in Chinese).
    Williamson M. Relationship of species number to area, distance and other variables. Dordrecht: Springer; 1988.
    Yuan Y, Zeng G, Liang J, Li X, Li Z, Zhang C, Huang L, Lai X, Lu L, Wu H. Effects of landscape structure, habitat and human disturbance on birds: a case study in East Dongting Lake wetland. Ecol Eng. 2014;67:67–75.
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(6)

    1. Ibrahim M. Ahmad, Dongming Li. More than a simple egg: Underlying mechanisms of cold tolerance in avian embryos. Avian Research, 2023, 14: 100104. DOI:10.1016/j.avrs.2023.100104
    2. Ava-Rose F Beech, Mattheus C Santos, Emily B Smith, et al. High within-clutch repeatability of eggshell phenotype in Barn Swallows despite less maculated last-laid eggs. Ornithology, 2022, 139(4) DOI:10.1093/ornithology/ukac024
    3. Joanna Rosenbeger, Kamil Pytlak, Ewa Łukaszewicz, et al. Variation in Bird Eggs—Does Female Factor, Season, and Laying Order Impact the Egg Size, Pigmentation, and Eggshell Thickness of the Eggs of Capercaillie?. Animals, 2021, 11(12): 3454. DOI:10.3390/ani11123454
    4. Longwu Wang, Gangbin He, Yuhan Zhang, et al. Cryptic eggs are rejected less frequently by a cuckoo host. Animal Cognition, 2021, 24(6): 1171. DOI:10.1007/s10071-021-01507-2
    5. Wei-Guo Du, Richard Shine, Liang Ma, et al. Adaptive responses of the embryos of birds and reptiles to spatial and temporal variations in nest temperatures. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2019, 286(1915): 20192078. DOI:10.1098/rspb.2019.2078
    6. Ping Ye, Canchao Yang, Wei Liang. Nest site availability and niche differentiation between two cavity‐nesting birds in time and space. Ecology and Evolution, 2019, 9(20): 11904. DOI:10.1002/ece3.5698

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(4)  /  Tables(2)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (255) PDF downloads (8) Cited by(6)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return