Zheng Li, Jie Zhou, Minzhi Gao, Wei Liang, Lu Dong. 2022: Parapatric speciation with recurrent gene flow of two sexual dichromatic pheasants. Avian Research, 13(1): 100031. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2022.100031
Citation: Zheng Li, Jie Zhou, Minzhi Gao, Wei Liang, Lu Dong. 2022: Parapatric speciation with recurrent gene flow of two sexual dichromatic pheasants. Avian Research, 13(1): 100031. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2022.100031

Parapatric speciation with recurrent gene flow of two sexual dichromatic pheasants

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    E-mail address: donglu@bnu.edu.cn (L. Dong)

  • Received Date: 12 Mar 2022
  • Accepted Date: 11 Apr 2022
  • Available Online: 06 Jul 2022
  • Publish Date: 19 Apr 2022
  • Understanding speciation has long been a fundamental goal of evolutionary biology. It is widely accepted that speciation requires an interruption of gene flow to generate strong reproductive isolation between species. The mechanism of how speciation in sexually dichromatic species operates in the face of gene flow remains an open question. Two species in the genus Chrysolophus, the Golden Pheasant (C. pictus) and Lady Amherst's Pheasant (C. amherstiae), both of which exhibit significant plumage dichromatism, are currently parapatric in southwestern China with several hybrid recordings in field. In this study, we estimated the pattern of gene flow during the speciation of the two pheasants using the Approximate Bayesian Computation (ABC) method based on data from multiple genes. Using a newly assembled de novo genome of Lady Amherst's Pheasant and resequencing of widely distributed individuals, we reconstructed the demographic history of the two pheasants by the PSMC (pairwise sequentially Markovian coalescent) method. The results provide clear evidence that the gene flow between the two pheasants was consistent with the predictions of the isolation with migration model during divergence, indicating that there was long-term gene flow after the initial divergence (ca. 2.2 million years ago). The data further support the occurrence of secondary contact between the parapatric populations since around 30 kya with recurrent gene flow to the present, a pattern that may have been induced by the population expansion of the Golden Pheasant in the late Pleistocene. The results of the study support the scenario of speciation between the Golden Pheasant and Lady Amherst's Pheasant with cycles of mixing-isolation-mixing, possibly due to the dynamics of geographical context in the late Pleistocene. The two species provide a good research system as an evolutionary model for testing reinforcement selection in speciation.

  • Interactions between brood parasites and their hosts are regarded as a model system for the study of coevolution (Davies 2000; Soler 2014). Brood parasites lay their eggs in hosts' nests and subsequently transfer the cost of parental care to the hosts. This selects for the evolution of defenses in hosts, which in turn selects for reciprocal counter-adaptations in the parasite. Although there is a vast literature on the reciprocal adaptations of brood parasites and their hosts at the pre-laying (Davies and Welbergen 2009; Feeney et al. 2012), egg (Davies and Brooke 1988; Starling et al. 2006; Spottiswoode and Stevens 2010; Yang et al. 2010, 2016a; Stoddard and Stevens 2011) and chick stages (Langmore et al. 2003; de Mársico et al. 2012; Yang et al. 2015b), less is known about how the coevolution between parasites and hosts reach the current outcome of these arms races (Kilner and Langmore 2011). In particular, relatively few studies have investigated whether host defenses can result in escaping from brood parasitism.

    Although the coevolutionary outcome of an arms race is difficult to elucidate strong indirect evidence has been found to demonstrate that some hosts have defeated brood parasites by maintaining strong anti-parasitism defenses such as egg rejection despite a lack of current parasitism. Such evidence has been obtained for several suspected former hosts of cuckoos. For example, the Hume's Leaf Warbler (Phylloscopus humei), which is not currently parasitized, shows high inter-clutch variation in egg size and rejects eggs that differ in size to their own clutch, thus preventing egg matching by the parasite and allowing successful prevention of parasitism (Marchetti 2000).

    Compared to Hume's Leaf Warbler, other potential hosts in Asia present more of a conundrum if they lack egg rejection behavior, yet they are not exploited by brood parasites. One explanation is that these hosts may have won the arms race by showing specific defenses at other stages (e.g., chick stage: Grim 2006) of the breeding cycle. For example, the Least Flycatchers (Empidonax minimus) experience rates of parasitism six times lower than sympatric Yellow Warblers (Dendroica petechial) (Briskie et al. 1990). This difference was attributed in large part to the more aggressive and effective nest defense by Least Flycatchers (Briskie et al. 1990). Although most studies have been done on coevolutionary interactions at the pre-laying or egg stages of the breeding cycle, hosts may also show defenses at the chick stage by deserting (Langmore et al. 2003; Grim 2007) or evicting (Sato et al. 2010; Yang et al. 2015b) alien nestlings. However, without studying several, and ideally all, breeding stages (e.g., pre-laying, egg, chick), it is hard to tell why some hosts escape parasitism or reduce its rate substantially (Briskie et al. 1990; Grim et al. 2011; Li et al. 2016).

    Sparrow species (Passer spp.) are widespread across the Old World and feed their offspring on insects (Baumgartner 1937; Seel 2008; Girard et al. 2012; Yang et al. 2015a), which should provide a suitable diet for cuckoo chicks. Generally their egg recognition capacities are absent or negligible and some of them (e.g. House Sparrow P. domesticus) were suggested to be unsuitable model species for egg rejection experiments on a global scale (Manna et al. 2017). However, they have never been used as hosts by any parasitic cuckoo (Moksnes and Røskaft 1995; Yang et al. 2012b) and till now this puzzle has not been reasonably explained. These include a variety of sparrow species such as Tree Sparrows, House Sparrows, and Russet Sparrow (Passer cinnamomeus). There have been two hypotheses that tried to explain this puzzle. First, hosts may escape parasitism by breeding in urban areas as a specific adaptation or a byproduct (Liang et al. 2013). Second, they may build nest in small cavities that exclude larger parasites (Davies 2000). However, none of these explanations presents a satisfactory solution to this puzzle. On one hand, some host species living in urban areas are exploited by brood parasites (Yang et al. 2012b). On the other hand, in addition to cavity nests, sparrows also build open nests in trees (Yang et al. 2015a), and most importantly cavity nests cannot prevent small parasites (e.g. Asian Emerald Cuckoo Chrysococcyx maculatus) from entering. Additionally, even medium to large sized cuckoos can enter nest boxes to parasitize cavity-nesting hosts, including Common Redstart (Phoenicurus phoenicurus) (Samaš et al. 2016), tits (Parus spp.) and flycatchers (Ficedula spp.) (Deng 2013; Grim et al. 2014; Liang et al. 2016). Therefore, all the current explanations cannot resolve why sparrows have never been utilized as hosts by any parasitic cuckoo, even though they are abundant and widespread.

    Here we conducted parasitic experiments at both egg and chick stages of the breeding cycle to investigate the egg and chick recognition abilities of Russet Sparrow, a widespread potential cuckoo host but not currently utilized by any parasitic cuckoos. Because nest sanitation was hypothesized to be a pre-adaptation of egg recognition (Rothstein 1975; Moskát et al. 2003) that hosts generally show stronger recognition toward non-egg-shaped objects than egg-shaped objects (Yang et al. 2015c), we also tested nest sanitation behavior in Russet Sparrows. This study aims to determine whether egg or chick stage defenses can explain the lack of parasitism of this species by brood parasites.

    Experiments on Russet Sparrows were conducted in Kuankuoshui (KKS) National Nature Reserve during April‒August 2013. KKS (28°10ʹN, 107°10ʹE) lies in a subtropical evergreen broadleaf forest in southwestern China. The mean annual temperature and precipitation are 13.4 ℃ and 1330 mm, respectively (Yang et al. 2010).

    The Old World sparrows are among the most familiar of all bird species (Allende et al. 2001). The Russet Sparrow, was formerly recognized as P. rutilans (Mlíkovský 2011; also see Yang et al. 2012a). The Russet Sparrow is a sexually dimorphic species and prefers light woodland, although they may sometimes be found in towns and agricultural areas (Yang et al. 2012a). They mainly use dry grasses for nesting and the nest structures depend on the cavity size they choose. In the present study all nests of Russet Sparrows for experiments were inside nest boxes.

    Artificial parasitism during the egg stage was performed in sparrow nests on the day after clutch completion or at the beginning of incubation. We artificially parasitized the nests with model eggs, models that were coin-shaped or stick-shaped, or real eggs of alien species from cross-fostering experiment (see below). The use of stick and coin models allowed us to test whether Russet Sparrow shows basic nest sanitation behavior, which is believed to be a pre-requisite for the evolution of egg rejection (Guigueno and Sealy 2012; Yang et al. 2015c). Each sparrow nest received one of the following treatments: (1) blue model eggs; (2) model coins; and (3) model sticks. Model eggs, model coins and model sticks were all made of polymer clay with a mass of ca. 1.45 g similar to but slightly lighter than that of sparrow eggs (1.78 ± 0.12 g, n = 10). Model coins were pie-shaped with a diameter of ca. 22 mm and ca. 6 mm in thickness while model sticks were cylinder-shaped with a length of ca. 21 mm and ca. 11 mm diameter in cross section. For each nest, one egg or model was inserted into the nests of sparrows, and they were monitored on a daily basis for 6 days. Results of the experiments were classified as acceptance (alien objects are warm and intact and being incubated with eggs) or rejection (alien objects gone or left cold in the nest) (Yang et al. 2010). No experimental nests were deserted (Table 1).

    Table  1.  Outcome of experimental parasitism in Russet Sparrows
    Parasite model egg Accepted Ejected Total
    Blue model egg 11 (100.0) 0 11
    Blue model coin 5 (27.8) 13 18
    Blue model stick 6 (50.0) 6 12
    Real alien egga 10 (100.0) 0 10
    aReal eggs of 10 alien species from cross-fostering experiment. Numbers in brackets refer to the percentage of acceptance (%)
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    In total alien eggs from 10 sympatric species were used in cross-fostering experiments for Russet Sparrow. Eggs (n = 15 nests) or newly hatched chicks (n = 2 nests) of similar mass were inserted into sparrow nests and their fate was monitored daily. We chose these 10 alien species because they are sympatric with Russet Sparrow in the same study area and their nests can be found during the breeding season of the sparrow. However, the choice is random and alien chicks with similar body mass as Russet Sparrow were used during the experiment. Nests of Russet Sparrow received one of three treatments: (1) cuckoo group-host nests were manipulated to contain one cuckoo chick; (2) foreign group-host nests were manipulated to contain two alien chicks of a non-cuckoo species; or (3) mixed group-each nest was manipulated to two non-cuckoo alien chicks and two host chicks. Considering the ethical problem of potential risk of death in alien chicks, for each alien species we only used two samples (i.e. two nests) or one sample (i.e. one nest). Furthermore, two kinds of control groups were included: (1) manipulated control-cross-fostering of the same sparrow species between different clutches; and (2) non-manipulated control-visits without cross-fostering manipulation. Small portable far infrared cameras (108.9 mm × 32.8 mm × 12.5 mm in size and 90 g in weight, JWD DV-58, Jingwah Digital Technology Co., Ltd., Shenzhen, China) were set up inside experimental nest boxes to monitor feeding frequency of host chicks and alien chicks by host parents. Feeding frequency was summarized from video records to investigate feeding preferences by sparrow parents if any existed. We recorded host provisioning rates for 48 h of video recordings for the cuckoo group and 30 h for the foreign group, and 108 h for the mixed group. For the mixed group, host chicks with parasites of Ashy-throated Parrobtill (Paradoxornis alphonsianus), Yellow-throated Bunting and Red-billed Leiothrix (Leiothrix lutea) were recorded. For the foreign group, video records included Brown-breasted Bulbul (Pycnonotus xanthorrhous), Green-backed Tit (Parus monticolus), Ashy-throated Parrotbill, and Red-billed Leiothrix. For the cuckoo group, Lesser Cuckoo (Cuculus poliocephalus) was recorded. The average feeding frequency per chick per hour was calculated for each observed nest and the results are shown in Fig. 3.

    Figure  3.  Feeding frequency of nestlings in the mixed (a) and cuckoo/foreign (b) groups of cross-fostering experiments. Each dot refers to the average feeding frequency per chick per hour of parasite or host in each observed nest

    IBM SPSS Statistics 20.0 for Windows (IBM Inc.) was used for the calculations and the data were presented as mean ± SD. The average feeding frequency of hosts and parasites in the mixed group was calculated and compared by paired sample t test. Scatter plots were generated for the feeding frequency with days to present the tendency of feeding.

    In response to artificial parasitism at the egg stage, Russet Sparrow accepted 100% of model eggs and real eggs (from cross-fostering experiments). By contrast, Russet Sparrows accepted 27.8% of model coins and 50% of model sticks. In the cross-fostering experiment, all dead chicks died in Russet Sparrow nests except one case of Tree Sparrow (Passer montanus) chick (Figs. 1, 2). No rejection behavior was detected in control groups. In two cases of the mixed groups, Russet Sparrow own chicks also died. All dead chicks died because of starvation or nest desertion except one case that a female sparrow ejected one live chick of yellow-throated bunting (Emberiza elegans) from its nest (Fig. 1). Survival time of rejected chicks varied from two to 14 days (Fig. 2). Only one case was confirmed as ejection because Russet Sparrows did not identify alien chicks during begging as alien chicks disappeared unexpectedly at any time during 13 days (two to 14 days) before we detected the disappearance.

    Figure  1.  Chick rejection behavior in Russet Sparrows. a Russet Sparrow nest with two bunting chicks (indicated by black arrows) and 2 Russet Sparrow chicks (between the two bunting chicks). b Russet Sparrow parents returned to the nest and started to hold a bunting chick in its beak. c Russet Sparrow parents ejected the bunting chick
    Figure  2.  Summary results for cross-fostering experiments in Russet Sparrows. Russet sparrows on Y-axis refer to host nestlings in mixed groups with parasite species above, respectively. Species with solid lines or without lines below indicate that they succeeded or failed to fledge, respectively. Species with dashed lines indicate that both circumstances (success or failure) exist

    In the cross-fostering experiment, host parents did not feed parasites for 2 days up to 15 days depending on parasite species (Fig. 2). Furthermore, the frequency of feeding of parasites in foreign and cuckoo groups decreased as they grew larger and finally hosts stopped feeding them (Fig. 3). However, host parents kept on feeding their own nestlings but abandoned alien nestlings in the mixed group. Therefore, the parasite chicks received much less food than host chicks in the mixed group and finally starve to death (t = 4.62, df = 4, p = 0.01, paired sample t test; Fig. 3).

    Our parasitism experiments showed that the Russet Sparrow has no egg recognition abilities. For house sparrows in Europe, although rejection of conspecific eggs was reported (Lopez de Hierro and Moreno-Rueda 2010; Soler et al. 2011; but see Yang et al. 2015a, 2016b), Manna et al. (2017) conducted parasitism experiments in different geographic populations and suggested that the House Sparrow rejection capacity was weak and negligible. In contrast, Russet Sparrow recognized and ejected model coins and sticks, which revealed a certain level of nest cleaning behavior, but lower than in host species with intermediate (e.g., Barn Swallow: 74 and 68% for coins and sticks respectively; Yang et al. 2015c) or strong (e.g., Great Reed Warbler Acrocephalus arundinaceus: 93.1 and 93.5% for coins and sticks respectively; Moskát et al. 2003) egg rejection capacity.

    Cross-fostering experiments indicated that Russet Sparrows have chick recognition abilities. According to our results, newly hatched alien chicks, which cannot produce begging calls yet, were ejected or starved to dead in sparrow nests, which implied that Russet Sparrow can identify alien chicks by visual cues. Generally in passerines, nestlings produce begging calls only after several days post-hatch (e.g., Colombelli-Négrel et al. 2012) and our observation is consistent with previous studies. However, they spend variable time of making a decision from 2 to 14 days. Therefore, both the visual and vocal contrasts between alien chicks and growing chicks of sparrows may both influence chick cognition in Russet Sparrow. Combined with previous studies, unlike egg recognition that occurs very fast (generally less than 3 days), chick recognition shows large variation (Langmore et al. 2003: 3‒6 days; Grim et al. 2003: ca. 14 days). Furthermore, Russet Sparrows reject alien chicks in all cross-fostered groups, which indicated that they do not need their own chicks as comparison (see also Grim 2006). Similarly, previous studies also indicated that generally hosts do not need their own eggs as comparison for alien egg rejection (Moskát and Hauber 2007; Wang et al. 2015, but see Yang et al. 2014). One case of three Tree Sparrows succeeded to fledge from a host nest, which may be explained by the similarity between their chicks because they are closely related sister-species (Jetz et al. 2012). Additionally, two cases of Russet Sparrows in mixed groups also failed to fledge, which implied that recognition error may exist. For example, Sato et al. (2010) reported chick recognition error in the Large-billed Gerygone (Gerygone magnirostris) host when they rejected the Little Bronze-cuckoo (Chrysococcyx minutillus) nestlings, whilst few studies revealed recognition error (Grim et al. 2003; Langmore et al. 2003; Yang et al. 2015b). Further studies are needed in the future to test these presumptions.

    Although initial study suggested that chick recognition is maladaptive and difficult to evolve (Lotem 1993), an increasing number of studies have shown that chick recognition can evolve as an adaptation toward brood parasitism (Grim et al. 2003; Langmore et al. 2003; Grim 2011; Yang et al. 2015b). However, chick recognition is not as common as egg recognition, and this may be explained by the rarer enemy hypothesis which suggests that chick recognition is prone to evolve only in hosts without egg recognition as a consequence of a release from a selection pressure against parasites at an earlier developmental stage, i.e., egg stage (Grim 2006).

    In summary, our studies showed that Russet Sparrows have no egg recognition ability, but recognize their own nestlings and eject alien chicks or starve them to death. By rejecting nestlings of foreign species, Russet Sparrows have succeeded to escape from the brood parasitism by cuckoos and other parasites.

    This study showed that Russet Sparrows have no egg recognition ability, but recognize their own nestlings and eject alien chicks or starve them to death. They may use visual cues in chick discrimination, although they accept sister species Tree Sparrows. By rejecting nestlings of foreign species, Russet Sparrows have succeeded to escape from the brood parasitism by cuckoos and other parasites. The present study shed light on the puzzle why some species are not utilized by cuckoo parasites as hosts, and perhaps that Asian hosts are becoming a great model for the study of parasite-host coevolution. In addition, our study highlighted the necessity to test in more host species for chick discrimination (ideally those that are known egg acceptors-see the rarer enemy effect, Grim 2006).

    WL and CY conceived and designed the experiment, JH and TS conducted the experiments and collected field data. APM provided new ideas for this manuscript. JH and CY performed data analyses and wrote an early version of the manuscript. WL and APM revised and improved the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

    We thank Junqiu Wu, Guoxian Liang, and Ping Ye for their assistance with fieldwork, and the help and cooperation from Kuankuoshui National Nature Reserves. We are grateful to two anonymous reviewers for providing constructive and valuable comments that significantly improved the quality of the manuscript.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    The datasets used in the present study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

    Not applicable.

    The experiments comply with the current laws of China where they were performed. Ethical concerns were given the highest priority and we kept sample sizes of the cross-fostering experiment to a minimum (Taborsky 2010; Grim et al. 2011). Experimental procedures were in agreement with the Animal Research Ethics Committee of Hainan Provincial Education Centre for Ecology and Environment, Hainan Normal University (permit no. HNECEE-2011-002). Fieldwork was carried out under the permission from Kuankuoshui National Nature Reserves, China. Experiments, including cross-fostering of chicks, were carried out following standard protocols widely used in similar studies (Yang et al. 2013, 2016b).

  • Anderson, E.C., Thompson, E.A., 2002. A model-based method for identifying species hybrids using multilocus genetic data. Genetics 160, 1217-1229
    Arrieta, R.S., Lijtmaer, D.A., Tubaro, P.L., 2013. Evolution of postzygotic reproductive isolation in galliform birds: analysis of first and second hybrid generations and backcrosses. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 110, 528-542
    Beaumont, M.A., 2010. Approximate Bayesian computation in evolution and ecology. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 41, 379-406
    Beaumont, M.A., Zhang, W., Balding, D.J., 2002. Approximate Bayesian computation in population genetics. Genetics 162, 2025-2035
    Beaumont, O., Bonichon, N., Duchon, P., Eyraud-Dubois, L., Larcheveque, H., 2008. A distributed algorithm for resource clustering in large scale platforms. In: Baker, T.P., Bui, A., Tixeuil, S. (Eds), Principles of Distributed Systems. OPODIS 2008. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 5401. Springer, Berlin, pp. 564-567
    Bertorelle, G., Benazzo, A., Mona, S., 2010. ABC as a flexible framework to estimate demography over space and time: some cons, many pros. Mol. Ecol. 19, 2609-2625
    Blum, M.G.B., Francois, O., 2010. Non-linear regression models for Approximate Bayesian Computation. Stat. Comput. 20, 63-73
    Bolnick, D.I., Fitzpatrick, B.M., 2007. Sympatric speciation: models and empirical evidence. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 38, 459-487
    Butler, J., MacCallum, I., Kleber, M., Shlyakhter, I.A., Belmonte, M.K., Lander, E.S., et al., 2008. ALLPATHS: de novo assembly of whole-genome shotgun microreads. Genome Res. 18, 810-820
    Cornejo-Romero, A., Vargas-Mendoza, C.F., Aguilar-Martinez, G.F., Medina-Sanchez, J., Rendon-Aguilar, B., Valverde, P.L., et al., 2017. Alternative glacial-interglacial refugia demographic hypotheses tested on Cephalocereus columna-trajani (Cactaceae) in the intertropical Mexican drylands. PLoS ONE 12, e0175905
    Coyne, J.A., Orr, H.A., 2004. Speciation. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland
    Csillery, K., Francois, O., Blum, M.G.B., 2012. Abc: an R package for approximate Bayesian computation (ABC). Method. Ecol. Evol. 3, 475-479
    Darriba, D., Taboada, G.L., Doallo, R., Posada, D., 2012. JModelTest 2: more models, new heuristics and parallel computing. Nat. Method. 9, 772
    Deng, Q.X., 1974. A natural hybrid of Chrysolophus amherstiae and C. picus. Curr. Zool. 20, 106-107. (in Chinese)
    Don, R.H., Cox, P.T., Wainwright, B.J., Baker, K., Mattick, J.S., 1991. “Touchdown” PCR to circumvent spurious priming during gene amplification. Nucleic Acids Res. 19, 4008
    Dong, F., Hung, C.M., Li, X.L., Gao, J.Y., Zhang, Q., Wu, F., et al., 2017. Ice age unfrozen: severe effect of the last interglacial, not glacial, climate change on East Asian avifauna. BMC Evol. Biol. 17, 244
    Dong, L., Heckel, G., Liang, W., Zhang, Y., 2013. Phylogeography of Silver Pheasant (Lophura nycthemera L.) across China: aggregate effects of refugia, introgression and riverine barriers. Mol. Ecol. 22, 3376-3390
    Drummond, A.J., Suchard, M.A., Xie, D., Rambaut, A., 2012. Bayesian phylogenetics with BEAUti and the BEAST 1.7. Mol. Biol. Evol. 29, 1969-1973
    Duchen, P., Zivkovic, D., Hutter, S., Stephan, W., Laurent, S., 2013. Demographic inference reveals African and European admixture in the North American Drosophila melanogaster population. Genetics 193, 291-301
    Fraser, C.I., Nikula, R., Ruzzante, D.E., Waters, J.M., 2012. Poleward bound: biological impacts of southern Hemisphere glaciation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 27, 462-471
    Funk, D.J., Nosil, P., Etges, W.J., 2006. Ecological divergence exhibits consistently positive associations with reproductive isolation across disparate taxa. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 103, 3209-3213
    He, J.L., Wei, L., 1993. Note on some natural hybrids of Chrysolophus amherstiae and C. picus from Dayi County, Sichuan. Zool. Res. 14, 239-240. (in Chinese)
    Hewitt, G., 2000. The genetic legacy of the quaternary ice ages. Nature 405, 907-913
    Hewitt, G.M., 2001. Speciation, hybrid zones and phylogeography - or seeing genes in space and time. Mol. Ecol. 10, 537-549
    Hewitt, G.M., 2004. Genetic consequences of climatic oscillations in the Quaternary. Philos. T. Roy. Soc. B. 359, 183-195
    Hudson, R.R., Kreitman, M., Aguade, M., 1987. A test of neutral molecular evolution based on nucleotide data. Genetics 116, 153-159
    Jean-Marie, C., Filipe, S., Mark, A.B., Christian, P.R., Jean-Michel, M., David, J.B., et al., 2008. Inferring population history with DIY ABC: a user-friendly approach to approximate Bayesian computation. Bioinformatics 24, 2713-2719
    Kirkpatrick, M., Servedio, M.R., 1999. The reinforcement of mating preferences on an island. Genetics 151, 865-884
    Lambeck, K., Chappell, J., 2001. Sea level change through the last glacial cycle. Science 292, 679-686
    Lamichhaney, S., Berglund, J., Almen, M.S., Maqbool, K., Grabherr, M., Martinez-Barrio, A., et al., 2015. Evolution of Darwin’s finches and their beaks revealed by genome sequencing. Nature 518, 371-375
    Li, H., 2013. Aligning sequence reads, clone sequences and assembly contigs with BWA-MEM. arXiv:1303.3997v2 [q-bio.GN]
    Li, H., Durbin, R., 2011. Inference of human population history from individual whole-genome sequences. Nature 475, 493-496
    Li, H., Handsaker, B., Wysoker, A., Fennell, T., Ruan, J., Homer, N., et al., 2009. The sequence alignment/map format and SAMtools. Bioinformatics 25, 2078-2079
    Li, J.W., Yeung, C.K.L., Tsai, P.W., Lin, R.C., Yeh, C.F., Yao, C.T., et al., 2010. Rejecting strictly allopatric speciation on a continental island: prolonged postdivergence gene flow between Taiwan (Leucodioptron taewanus, Passeriformes, Timaliidae) and Chinese (L. canorum canorum) hwameis. Mol. Ecol. 19, 494-507
    Librado, P., Rozas, J., 2009. DnaSP v5: a software for comprehensive analysis of DNA polymorphism data. Bioinformatics 25, 1451-1452
    Liu, Y., Liu, S., Yeh, C.F., Zhang, N., Chen, G., Que, P., et al., 2018. The first set of universal nuclear protein-coding loci markers for avian phylogenetic and population genetic studies. Sci. Rep. 8, 15723
    Lyu, N., Packert, M., Tietze, D.T., Sun, Y.H., 2015. Uncommon paleodistribution patterns of Chrysolophus pheasants in east Asia: explanations and implications. J. Avian Biol. 46, 528-537
    Mayr, E., 1942. Systematics and the Origin of Species from the Viewpoint of a Zoologist. Columbia University Press, New York
    Nosil, P., 2012. Ecological Speciation. Oxford University Press, Oxford
    Nosil, P., 2008. Speciation with gene flow could be common. Mol. Ecol. 17, 2103-2106
    Olsen, J.B., Crane, P.A., Flannery, B.G., Dunmall, K., Templin, W.D., Wenburg, J.K., 2011. Comparative landscape genetic analysis of three Pacific salmon species from subarctic North America. Conserv. Genet. 12, 223-241
    Pavlidis, P., Laurent, S., Stephan, W., 2010. MsABC: a modification of Hudson’s ms to facilitate multi-locus ABC analysis. Mol. Ecol. Resour. 10, 723-727
    Peng, M.S., Wu, F., Murphy, R.W., Yang, X.J., Zhang, Y.P., 2016. An ancient record of an avian hybrid and the potential uses of art in ecology and conservation. Ibis 158, 444-445
    Poelstra, J.W., Vijay, N., Bossu, C.M., Lantz, H., Ryll, B., Muller, I., et al., 2014. The genomic landscape underlying phenotypic integrity in the face of gene flow in crows. Science 344, 1410-1414
    Provan, J., Bennett, K.D., 2008. Phylogeographic insights into cryptic glacial refugia. Trends Ecol. Evol. 23, 564-571
    Rambaut, A., Drummond, A.J., 2009. Tracer v 1.5.
    R Core Team, 2020. A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. URL
    Rice, W.R., Salt, G.W., 1988. Speciation via disruptive selection on habitat preference: experimental evidence. Am. Nat. 131, 911-917
    Rohling, E.J., Fenton, M., Jorissen, F.J., Bertrand, P., Ganssen, G., Caulet, J.P., 1998. Magnitudes of sea-level lowstands of the past 500,000 years. Nature 394, 162-165
    Servedio, M.R., 2000. Reinforcement and the genetics of nonrandom mating. Evolution 54, 21-29
    Servedio, M.R., 2004. The what and why of research on reinforcement. PLoS Biol. 2, e420
    Servedio, M.R., 2011. Limits to the evolution of assortative mating by female choice under restricted gene flow. P. Roy. Soc. B. Biol. Sci. 278, 179-187
    Servedio, M.R., 2012. The relationship between sexual selection and speciation. Curr. Zool. 58, 413-415
    Servedio, M.R., 2016. Geography, assortative mating, and the effects of sexual selection on speciation with gene flow. Evol. Appl. 9, 91-102
    Servedio, M.R., Burger, R., 2014. The counterintuitive role of sexual selection in species maintenance and speciation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 111, 8113-8118
    Servedio, M.R., Kirkpatrick, M., 1997. The effects of gene flow on reinforcement. Evolution 51, 1764-1772
    Servedio, M.R., Noor, M.A.F., 2003. The role of reinforcement in speciation: theory and data. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 34, 339-364
    Shafer, A.B.A., Cullingham, C.I., Cote, S.D., Coltman, D.W., 2010. Of glaciers and refugia: A decade of study sheds new light on the phylogeography of northwestern North America. Mol. Ecol. 19, 4589-4621
    Shi, X.Y., Fu, Q., Wang, L., Jiang, Z.Y., Shi, X.G., Li, S., 2018. Note on the natural hybridization of Golden Pheasant (Chrysolophus pictus) and Lady Amherst’s Pheasant (C. amherstiae) in Anzihe Nature Reserve, Sichuan Province. Chinese J Zool. 53, 660-663. (in Chinese)
    Tajima, F., 1989. Statistical method for testing the neutral mutation hypothesis by DNA polymorphism. Genetics 123, 585-595
    Tamura, K., Stecher, G., Peterson, D., Filipski, A., Kumar, S., 2013. MEGA6: molecular evolutionary genetics analysis version 6.0. Mol. Biol. Evol. 30, 2725-2729
    Thompson, J.D., Higgins, D.G., Gibson, T.J., 1994. CLUSTAL W: improving the sensitivity of progressive multiple sequence alignment through sequence weighting, position-specific gap penalties and weight matrix choice. Nucleic Acids Res. 22, 4673-4680
    Wang, P., Liu, Y., Liu, Y., Chang, Y., Wang, N., Zhang, Z., 2017. The role of niche divergence and geographic arrangement in the speciation of Eared Pheasants (Crossoptilon, Hodgson 1938). Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 113, 1-8
    Wang, P., Yeh, C., Chang, J., Yao, H., Fu, Y., Yao, C., et al., 2021. Multilocus phylogeography and ecological niche modeling suggest speciation with gene flow between the two Bamboo Partridges. Avian Res. 12, 17
    Wang, W., Dai, C., Alstrom, P., Zhang, C., Qu, Y., Li, S.H., et al., 2014. Past hybridization between two East Asian long-tailed tits (Aegithalos bonvaloti and A. fuliginosus). Front. Zool. 11, 40
    Wasserman, T.N., Cushman, S.A., Schwartz, M.K., Wallin, D.O., 2010. Spatial scaling and multi-model inference in landscape genetics: martes americana in northern Idaho. Landscape Ecol. 25, 1601-1612
    Wei, C., Dong, L., Li, S.H., Alstrom, P., Liu, Y., Xia, C., et al., 2019. From the Himalayas to a continental Island: Integrative species delimitation in the Brownish-flanked Bush Warbler Horornis fortipes complex. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 131, 219-227
    Zhang, D., Tang, L., Cheng, Y., Hao, Y., Xiong, Y., Song, G., et al., 2019. “Ghost Introgression” as a cause of deep mitochondrial divergence in a bird species complex. Mol. Biol. Evol. 36, 2375-2386
    Zhang, G., Li, C., Li, Q., Li, B., Larkin, D.M., Lee, C., et al., 2014. Comparative genomics reveals insights into avian genome evolution and adaptation. Science 346, 1311-1320
    Zhou, S.Z., Jijun, L., Zhang, S.Q., Zhao, J.D., Cui, J.X., 2004. Quaternary glaciations in China. Dev. Quat. Sci. 2, 105-113
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(14)

    1. Yuhan Zhang, Guo Zhong, Longwu Wang, et al. Nestling retrieval behavior in two bunting hosts of the common cuckoo. Journal of Ethology, 2025. DOI:10.1007/s10164-025-00836-w
    2. Sidhant Jain, Mallikarjun N. Shakarad. Adoption and foster parenting: an evolutionary enigma?. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 2024, 36(1): 1. DOI:10.1080/03949370.2023.2225032
    3. Hee-Jin Noh, Ros Gloag, Naomi E. Langmore. Multiple parasitism promotes facultative host acceptance of cuckoo eggs and rejection of cuckoo chicks. Animal Behaviour, 2023, 202: 1. DOI:10.1016/j.anbehav.2023.05.004
    4. Jing‐Chia Guo, Jo‐Szu Tsai, Jhih‐Syuan Wang, et al. The role of a synanthropic bird in the nest niche expansion of a secondary cavity nester to man‐made structures. Ecology and Evolution, 2022, 12(8) DOI:10.1002/ece3.9188
    5. Jörn Theuerkauf, Camila P. Villavicencio, Nicolas M. Adreani, et al. Austral birds offer insightful complementary models in ecology and evolution. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 2022, 37(9): 759. DOI:10.1016/j.tree.2022.05.003
    6. Qihong Li, Jianli Bi, Jiangwen Wu, et al. Impact of nest sanitation behavior on hosts’ egg rejection: an empirical study and meta-analyses. Current Zoology, 2021, 67(6): 683. DOI:10.1093/cz/zoab057
    7. Canchao Yang. Personality, recognition cues, and nest sanitation in obligate avian brood parasitism: what do we know and what comes next?. Current Zoology, 2021, 67(6): 621. DOI:10.1093/cz/zoab079
    8. Jiaojiao WANG, Qihong LI, Longwu WANG, et al. Do swallows (Hirundo daurica) use the visual cue of hatchling down‐feathers to discriminate parasite alien nestlings?. Integrative Zoology, 2020, 15(5): 441. DOI:10.1111/1749-4877.12436
    9. Miyu SASAKI, Haruna SUZUKI, Masao TAKAHASHI, et al. Breeding philopatry and natal philopatry of Russet Sparrow <i>Passer cinnamomeus</i> breeding in lowland open forest in eastern Aomori Prefecture. The Bulletin of the Japanese Bird Banding Association, 2020, 32(1_2): 12. DOI:10.14491/jbba.MS126
    10. M. Polačik, M. Reichard, C. Smith, et al. Parasitic cuckoo catfish exploit parental responses to stray offspring. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2019, 374(1769): 20180412. DOI:10.1098/rstb.2018.0412
    11. Jianping Liu, Cheng Cheng, Wei Liang. Egg recognition and chick discrimination in colonial breeding birds. Behavioural Processes, 2019, 168: 103955. DOI:10.1016/j.beproc.2019.103955
    12. Laikun Ma, Canchao Yang, Wei Liang. Hawk mimicry does not reduce attacks of cuckoos by highly aggressive hosts. Avian Research, 2018, 9(1) DOI:10.1186/s40657-018-0127-4
    13. Juan Huo, Tongping Su, Nan Niu, et al. Last but not the least: effects of laying sequence on egg color variation and embryonic development of Russet Sparrow (Passer cinnamomeus). Avian Research, 2018, 9(1) DOI:10.1186/s40657-018-0113-x
    14. Tomáš Grim. Avian Brood Parasitism. Fascinating Life Sciences, DOI:10.1007/978-3-319-73138-4_29

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(3)  /  Tables(1)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (294) PDF downloads (13) Cited by(14)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return