Iftikhar HUSSAIN, Asmat-un-NISA, Sangum KHALIL. 2012: Population biology of the Grey Francolin (Francolinus pondicerianus) in an agro-ecosystem of the Pothwar Plateau, Pakistan. Avian Research, 3(2): 91-102. DOI: 10.5122/cbirds.2012.0009
Citation: Iftikhar HUSSAIN, Asmat-un-NISA, Sangum KHALIL. 2012: Population biology of the Grey Francolin (Francolinus pondicerianus) in an agro-ecosystem of the Pothwar Plateau, Pakistan. Avian Research, 3(2): 91-102. DOI: 10.5122/cbirds.2012.0009

Population biology of the Grey Francolin (Francolinus pondicerianus) in an agro-ecosystem of the Pothwar Plateau, Pakistan

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Iftikhar HUSSAIN, E-mail: ifthussain@uaar.edu.pk

  • Received Date: 20 Sep 2011
  • Accepted Date: 19 Mar 2012
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2023
  • The field biology of the Grey Francolin (Francolinus pondicerianus) was studied in 2009 in an agro-ecosystem of the Pothwar area, an important range of its distribution in Pakistan. The bird density was estimated as 1.59±0.39 (mean±standard error) birds per ha in crop fields and 0.87±0.14 birds per ha in forest tracks, with some seasonal fluctuations in its habitat. All bird nests were located on the ground in vegetation mainly comprised of Desmostachia bipinnata, Acacia modesta, Imperata cylindrical, Zizipus jujuba and Euphorbia spp. Egg laying was recorded during the spring and summer months, with a mean egg laying span of 6±0.36 days (range 5-7 days) and a mean clutch size of 7±0.36 (range of 6-8 eggs). The mean incubation period was 20.6±0.50 days (range 19-22 days). Out of a total number of 42 eggs recorded, 32 hatched (76.19% success) with a mean hatching rate of 5.33±1.22 eggs per clutch. The fledgling success was estimated as 3.83±0.83 per clutch (63.08%). The species was found to be omnivorous and euryphagus in its feeding habit. There were 10 identifiable food items in the diet of the species, consisting of seven plant species (Pennisetum typhoideum, Sorghum bicolor, S. halepense, Prosopis juliflora, Phaseolus radiates, Carthemus axycantha and Acacia sp.), two insect species (Coptotermes formosanus and Formica rufa) and one grit.

  • The confrontation between adaptation and counter-adaptation of specific avian brood parasites, such as cuckoos, and their hosts, is a classic example of co-evolution (Davies 2000, 2011; Soler 2014). The high costs of successful cuckoo parasitism have forced hosts to evolve counter-adaptive strategies in different stages. For instance, during the egg laying and incubation stage, many hosts have developed the ability to recognize foreign eggs (Davies and Brooke 1988, 1989; Brooke and Davies 1988; Stokke et al. 2002; Spottiswoode and Stevens 2010, 2011; Yang et al. 2016). During the nestling stage, hosts are able to identify foreign chicks (Langmore et al. 2003, 2009; Grim 2007; Sato et al. 2010; Yang et al. 2015; Huo et al. 2018). Usually, cuckoos take one host egg during parasitism (Chance 1940; Davies 2000), which means that, once cuckoo eggs are laid, hosts suffer various levels of reproductive losses, even if they perform successful defense behaviors afterwards. Therefore, preventing cuckoos from laying eggs, i.e. nest defense, is the most effective anti-parasitism strategy (Welbergen and Davies 2009; Feeney et al. 2012).

    The competition between hosts and brood parasites during the egg laying and incubation stage has received much attention. Many hosts demonstrate strong aggressive nest protection behaviors against cuckoos approaching the nests, to prevent parasitism (Welbergen and Davies 2009). This leads to huge costs for the cuckoos (Davies and Brooke 1988; Welbergen and Davies 2008), and may even result in the death of the cuckoos (Molnár 1944). In addition, hosts' nest defenses against cuckoos not only prevent cuckoo parasitism (Welbergen and Davies 2009), but such defense behaviors can be passed on to other individuals through learning, hence strengthening anti-parasitism of the population (Davies and Welbergen 2009; Thorogood and Davies 2012, 2016; Feeney and Langmore 2013).

    Successful nest defense forces cuckoos to develop more effective parasitism strategies, and mimicking hawks (Accipiter spp.) is thought to be an adaption of cuckoos against host attacks (Langmore and Kilner 2010; Welbergen and Davies 2011; Feeney et al. 2012; Thorogood and Davies 2013a, b). Hawks are typical small birds of prey, and pose more direct threats to adult birds (Götmark and Post 1996). Resemblance to hawks enables cuckoos to confuse and alarm hosts, and reduce attacks from hosts (Davies and Welbergen 2008; Welbergen and Davies 2011). A recent study discovered that calls of female common cuckoos mimic those of sparrowhawks to some extent, which resulted in similar intimidation (York and Davies 2017). However, the effectiveness of cuckoos' hawk mimicry has only been demonstrated for a few host species, among which only reed warblers (Acrocephalus scirpaceus) are main hosts of common cuckoos (Cuculus canorus) (Davies and Welbergen 2008; Langmore and Kilner 2010; Welbergen and Davies 2011; Thorogood and Davies 2013b; Liang 2017; York and Davies 2017). Reed warblers lack aggressiveness, and are not strong nest defenders, although nest defense levels in reed warblers vary significantly between different species (Montgomerie and Weatherhead 1988). Therefore, results from studies on reed warblers cannot be extrapolated to other hosts. In fact, many hosts can accurately identify the differences between cuckoos and sparrowhawks, and respond with different defense levels (Duckworth 1991; Grim 2005; Welbergen and Davies 2008; Campobello and Sealy 2010; Feeney et al. 2012; Trnka and Prokop 2012; Li et al. 2015; Liang and Møller 2015; Yu et al. 2016, 2017). For example, Smith and Hosking (1955) discovered that many small passerines (not the main hosts of cuckoos) fear sparrowhawk dummies, but attack cuckoo dummies aggressively, suggesting their abilities to distinguish the two. Paired experiments with great reed warblers (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) by Trnka and Prokop (2012) revealed that, when presented simultaneously with paired dummies (of common cuckoo, sparrowhawk Accipiter nisus, and turtle dove Streptopelia turtur), great reed warblers exhibit aggressive attack behaviors to both dummies, with higher attack intensities towards cuckoos and sparrowhawks. However, a study by Feeney et al. (2015) suggested that tawny-flanked prinia (Prinia subflava) hosts were equally aggressive towards female cuckoo finches (Anomalospiza imberbis) and southern red bishops (Euplectes orix), suggesting successful mimicry of female bishops by female cuckoo finches.

    The oriental reed warbler (Acrocephaus orientalis) is one of the main hosts of common cuckoos, and the two have reached a relatively high "arms race level" after long-term coevolution (Yang et al. 2014, 2016, 2017; Li et al. 2016). Oriental reed warblers are highly aggressive towards intruders, and can adjust their attack intensities according to the level of threat and familiarity of the intruders (Li et al. 2015, 2016) or under variable parasitism pressures (Lindholm and Thomas 2000). In great reed warblers, they attacked cuckoos and sparrowhawks more often than turtle doves. However, when confronted with the simultaneously presented mounts, they attacked the cuckoo more frequently than the sparrowhawk (Trnka and Prokop 2012). In addition, Trnka and Prokop (2012) found that attack behaviors of great reed warblers towards common cuckoos weaken with nest stage (but see Moskát 2005).

    In this study, we tested a different population of oriental reed warblers during the egg laying, early incubation and nestling stages of nest cycle. Oriental reed warblers were presented with three different types of single dummies, namely common cuckoos, sparrowhawks and oriental turtle doves (Streptopelia orientalis), as well as paired dummies prepared by combining different dummies. By conducting these experiments, we aimed to evaluate the ability of oriental reed warblers to correctly identify potential brood parasites, and to assess nest defense behaviors towards different nest intruders.

    The study area is located in the Yongnianwa wetland in Yongnian County, Hebei Province, China (36°40′60″‒36°41′06″N, 114°41′15″-114°45′00″E). The Yongnianwa wetland is a natural depression in the alluvial plain of the Fuyang River, a tributary of the Hai River. It is located at the intersection of the Fuyang River and the Zhang River. The area is in a temperate semi-humid continental monsoon climate zone and includes mature hydrological systems with many river branches. The wetland is located at only 40.3 m above sea level, and is filled with water all year round. The average annual rainfall is 527.8 mm which concentrated in the summer and the annual average temperature is 12.9 ℃. The main plant species in the wetland are reed (Phragmites australis) mixed with cattail (Typha latifolia), and other herbaceous plants (Ma et al. 2018). A total of 257 oriental reed warbler nests with an overall parasitism rate of 14.8% were recorded during the breeding seasons 2016‒2017 (Ma et al. 2018).

    Field work was carried out during the bird breeding seasons (April-August) of 2016 and 2017. Breeding nests of oriental reed warblers were systematically searched within the study area, and the breeding stage of each nest was determined. Oriental reed warbler nest defense behaviors were investigated using dummies (stuffed birds) of common cuckoos (27.7 cm in body length), sparrowhawks (33.2 cm in body length), and oriental turtle doves (25.8 cm in body length). Common cuckoos are common brood parasites in the study area. Sparrowhawks are typical predators of passerine birds and are common summer breeding hawks in northern China (Zheng 2017). Oriental turtle doves are commonly seen breeding birds in the study area. Two dummies were taken from each bird species and one of them was randomly selected in the experiments in order to avoid pseudo-replication. Hosts' responses to the dummies included the following scale, from low to high levels of aggressiveness: (1) no response, the bird was not observed or watched the mount silently without clear responsive behavior; (2) alert, the bird approached the nest (hopped onto the reed stems), but gave alert calls from a safe distance (more than 1 m); (3) mobbing, the bird jumped or flew around/over the dummy, persistently giving alarm and distress calls; and (4) attack, the birds physically attacked the dummy. The first two responses are collectively considered to be non-attack behaviors, and the latter two are collectively considered to be attack behaviors (also see Li et al. 2015). The responsive behavior of the bird was recorded by L.M. hidden approximately 4‒5 m away from the nest site and other variables like number of attacks were extracted from subsequent analyses of video recordings in the laboratory. The mini-camera (size: 57 mm × 20 mm × 18 mm) was fixed at a distance of approximately 2 m in front of the nest and dummy.

    All single dummy experiments were completed during the 2016 breeding season. One dummy was randomly selected before an experiment. It was fixed at a distance of 0.5 m in front of an oriental reed warbler nest with a stand, and was kept standing with its head facing the nest. The 5-min responses of oriental reed warblers to the dummy were observed and recorded when the bird appeared around the dummy (also see Li et al. 2015). Subsequently, the next experiment was carried out at least 1 h later to minimize the effects of carry-over aggression (Gill et al. 2008). Each nest was presented with three mounts in random order and the sequence was completed within 1‒2 days.

    The three types of dummies were combined in pairs as follows: common cuckoo versus turtle dove (Experiment 1), common cuckoo versus sparrowhawk (Experiment 2) and sparrowhawk versus turtle dove (Experiment 3). One pair of dummies was randomly selected before an experiment, and one direction of an oriental reed warbler nest was randomly chosen. The two dummies, placed 0.8 m away from each other, were fixed at a distance of 0.5 m in front of the nest with two stands, and were kept standing with their heads facing the nest (also see Trnka et al. 2012). The responses of oriental reed warblers to the dummies were observed. Each experiment began at the moment of the first contact-attack of one bird and lasted 1 min when the mounts were immediately removed to prevent them from being damaged. The attack intensity towards each of the two dummies was expressed in relation to the absolute number of attacks within 1 min since the first attack which was extracted from subsequent analyses of video recordings in the laboratory. Only one experiment was performed for each nest either in egg stage or nestling stage.

    Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 16.0. For the single dummy experiment, generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) were used to analyze the effect of dummy types and nest stage on defense behaviors of oriental reed warblers. For the paired dummy experiment, GLMMs were used to analyze the effects of nest stage on the defense levels of oriental reed warblers. Wilcoxon signed ranks test was used to investigate variations in defense levels in response to different dummies in each paired experiment. The significance level was set at p = 0.05, and variables were considered to be highly significant when p < 0.01.

    In total, 39 dummy experiments were performed (Fig. 1), including two sets of single dummy experiments for four nests which one nest of egg stage was lack of hawk dummy experiment and three nests of nestling stage were lack of two hawk dummy experiments and one cuckoo dummy experiment, and three sets of single dummy experiments for the remaining nests. Thus, single dummy experiments were performed for 31 nests of oriental reed warblers, with 13 nests in the egg incubation stage, and 18 nests in the nestling stage.

    Figure  1.  Aggressive responses of oriental reed warblers to different types of dummies in the single dummy experiment

    All oriental reed warblers performed for single dummy experiments showed strong nest defense behaviors, and 90% (28/31) of the oriental reed warblers attacked the dummies. Attacks were observed in 73% of all experiments (65/89), and 69% of the attacks involved neighbor participation (45/65), with the number of helpers ranging from 1 to 11. Nest defense behaviors were not affected by dummy type nor nest stage (dummy type: F = 2.351, p = 0.102; nest stage: F = 0.000, p = 1.000).

    In total, 63 nests of oriental reed warblers were tested for paired dummy experiments either in egg stage or in nestling stage. The experiment 1 was performed for 7 nests in the egg stage and 12 nests in the nestling stage. The experiment 2 was performed for 11 nests in the egg stage and 11 nests in the nestling stage. The experiment 3 was performed for 10 nests in the egg stage and 11 nests in the nestling stage.

    Attack intensities of oriental reed warblers towards paired dummies did not vary with nest stage (cuckoo vs. dove: F = 0.069, p = 0.794; cuckoo vs. hawk: F = 0.061, p = 0.807; hawk vs. dove: F = 0.118, p = 0.735). Therefore, all data from the egg incubation and nestling stages were combined for analysis. There was a significant difference in the number of attacks directed to each dummy in the dummy combination of common cuckoo versus turtle dove, and the number of attacks directed at common cuckoos was significantly higher than that to turtle doves (Wilcoxon signed ranks test, Z = - 3.529, p < 0.001). However, we found no significant attack preferences for the other dummy pairs (Fig. 2; cuckoo vs. hawk: Z = - 1.038, p = 0.299; hawk vs. dove: Z = - 0.035, p = 0.972).

    Figure  2.  Intensity of aggressive responses of oriental reed warblers to different types of dummies in the paired dummy experiment

    The main findings of this study were that oriental reed warblers displayed aggressive nest defense behaviors with more than 90% of the warblers demonstrating strong attack behaviors towards intruders, and their nest defense behaviors did not differ between egg laying, incubation, and nestling stages. Our results were similar to previous work on reed warblers, which found few differences in overall attacks at different stages (Li et al. 2015), although rasp call rate towards cuckoos declined significantly between egg laying and chick rearing stages (Campobello and Sealy 2010). In addition, long time interval among different dummy experiments would minimize the effects of carry-over aggression.

    However, the results on the attack intensities of oriental reed warblers towards different types of attackers are not in agreement with the results found in another population of oriental reed warblers (Li et al. 2015) and great reed warblers (Trnka et al. 2012). In our study, oriental reed warblers were able to distinguish common cuckoos from harmless turtle doves, and tended to be more likely to mob cuckoos than sparrowhawks, although this tendency was not significant perhaps due to the low sample size for each presentation. These results imply that oriental reed warblers are highly aggressive towards any model placed at their nests regardless of mimicry.

    Many hosts are known to violently expel and attack cuckoos near their nests, causing different levels of damage to cuckoos (Wyllie 1981; Davies and Brooke 1988; Welbergen and Davies 2008), and even death (Molnár 1944). This reduces the chances of successful cuckoo parasitism (Welbergen and Davies 2009; Feeney et al. 2012; Thorogood and Davies 2016). The strong attacks of oriental reed warblers towards cuckoo dummies in our study resemble those of other hosts. Meanwhile, in paired experiments, when facing familiar intruders, namely common cuckoos and turtle doves, simultaneously, oriental reed warblers demonstrated significant selective aggression towards common cuckoos, indicating their abilities to distinguish the two intruders. This is in accordance with previous research on oriental reed warblers (Li et al. 2015).

    However, in both single and paired dummy experiments, the responses of oriental reed warblers to sparrowhawks were not significantly different from their responses to common cuckoos (Z = - 1.038, p = 0.299) and doves (Z = - 0.035, p = 0.972). In particular, in the paired dummy experiments, cuckoos were attacked much less often when they were presented alongside a hawk than a dove. This was different from the behaviors displayed by oriental reed warblers in the population studied by Li et al. (2015), as well as the great reed warbler populations of European lineage (Moskát 2005; Trnka and Prokop 2012; Trnka et al. 2012), suggesting that oriental reed warblers in our population may be deterred from mobbing overall by the presence of the predatory hawk. Interestingly, Li et al. (2015) also found that the magpie (Pica pica), a commonly seen but larger nest/egg predator, were attacked much less often by oriental reed warblers.

    In reed warblers, hosts' mobbing rates varied depending on cuckoo parasitism risk (Thorogood and Davies 2013a), and also by the risk of encountering a hawk if mistaking it for a cuckoo (Thorogood and Davies 2012, 2013b). In our study population of oriental reed warblers, the cuckoo parasitism rate was 14.8% (Ma et al. 2018), which was lower than that of other populations in China (27.3%; Yang et al. 2014), great reed warblers (64%; Moskát and Honza 2002) and reed warblers (31%; Trnka and Prokop 2011) in Europe. Such low parasitism might explain why oriental reed warblers displayed aggressive nest defense behaviors towards cuckoos suggesting that hawk mimicry may be ineffective in our population of oriental reed warblers.

    Another possibility was that oriental reed warblers are aggressive in general, and tend to expel any intruders. Our results indicated that even for familiar yet harmless doves, oriental reed warblers exhibited an attack rate as high as 58% (18/31), and even attacked people who approached their nests (L.M., personal observations). Among all attack behaviors, helper-assisted attacks occur in up to 69% of the nests studied. We suggest that the presence of helpers may reinforce the oriental reed warblers' desires to attack, and make them much bolder when facing intruders. In a study on European pied flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca), the existence of helpers was found to enhance European pied flycatchers' aggression and their desire to attack predators, and birds may sense the presence of helpers when attacking intruders (Krams et al. 2009, 2012). In our study, the presence of helpers within the population is much higher than in the population in other studies (Li et al. 2015, 2016), which may be due to different population densities and parasitism rates in different areas. Nonetheless, further studies are needed to investigate whether this may lead to variations in nest defense behaviors.

    In a European study on great reed warblers, it was discovered that their attack behaviors towards common cuckoos weaken with nest stage (Trnka and Prokop 2012). This is because, although common cuckoos may visit host nests at any stage, most eggs are laid during the egg laying stage, and parasitism pressure is reduced during the nestling stage (Honza et al. 2002). Other studies, however, suggested that great reed warblers' nest defense did not weaken with the advancement of nest stage, and that the attack intensities remained relatively high during the nestling stage (Moskát 2005). In our study, no changes in nest defense levels with the nest stages were found for oriental reed warblers, which agreed with previous work (Campobello and Sealy 2010; Li et al. 2015). Relative to the less aggressive hosts, the high aggressiveness of oriental reed warblers may be a broad nest defense behavior and thus does not vary with nest stage and intruder types. Another opinion is that cuckoos are not only nest parasites, but also nest predators, which has been reported for a number of host species (Davies and Brooke 1988; Moksnes et al. 2000; Briskie 2007; Su et al. 2017).

    In this study, we showed that oriental reed warblers not only exhibit strong nest defense behaviors, but that they also employ a variety of defenses intended to expel and attack any intruders within their nest-sites. Furthermore, assistance from conspecific helpers may increase oriental reed warblers' attack intensities. In addition, nest defense behaviors in oriental reed warblers did not change with breeding stage (i.e., egg stage, nestling stage). Our study indicated that oriental reed warblers are able to distinguish cuckoos from harmless doves. However, they may be deterred from mobbing by the presence of the predatory hawk, suggesting hawk mimicry may be ineffective and does not reduce attacks of cuckoos by highly aggressive hosts.

    WL conceived and designed the experiments. LM conducted the field work. CY and LM performed the data analysis. LM wrote the early draft and WL revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

    We would like to thank Rose Thorogood and the anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on an early version of this manuscript. We thank the Forestry Bureau of Yongnian County, Hebei Province, China, for permission to undertake this study including all experimental procedures. We are grateful to Jianping Liu, Jianwei Zhang, Bo Zhou and Xiaodong Rao for their assistance with fieldwork.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

    Not applicable.

    The experiments comply with the current laws of China. Experimental procedures were in agreement with the Animal Research Ethics Committee of Hainan Provincial Education Centre for Ecology and Environment, Hainan Normal University (Permit No. HNECEE-2012-003).

  • Ahmad M. 1990. Groundnut in the Dry Land Agriculture of Pothwar. Barani Agricultural Research and Development Project, PARC, Islamabad, Pakistan, p 43.
    Ali S, Ripley SD. 1983. A Pictorial Guide to the Birds of the Indian Subcontinent. Oxford University Press, New York, p 35.
    Ali S, Ripley SD. 1969. Handbook of the Birds of India and Paksitan. Vol. 3. Oxford University Press, Bombay, India.
    Baker EC. 1921. Indian Game Birds, Pheasants, Bustard, Quail. Bombay Nat Hist Soc, London, UK.
    Baker ECS. 1935. The Nidification of Birds of the Indian Empire. Taylor and Francis, London, p 4.
    Beg AR, Baig MS, Ali Q, Khan CMA. 1985. Agro-ecological zonation of Potwar: a-wheat, b-maize. National Agricultural Research Centre, Islamabad, Pakistan Forest Institute, Peshawar and Soil Survey of Pakistan, Lahore.
    Beg MA, Qureshi JI. 1972. Birds and their habitats in the cultivated areas of Lyallpur district and vicinity. Pakistan J Agric Sci, 9: 161–166.
    Bro E, Mayot P, Corda E, Reitz F. 2004. Impact of habitat management on grey partridge populations: Assessing wildlife cover using a multisite BACI experiment. J Appl Ecol, 41: 846–857.
    Bump G, Bump JW. 1964. A study and review of the Black Francolin and the Grey Francolin. Special Scientific Report Wildlife (81): Bureau of Sport Fisheries and Wildlife, Washington D.C., USA 81: 53–57.
    Burnham KP, Anderson DR, Laake JL. 1980. Estimation of density from line transects sampling of biological populations. Wildl Monogr, 72: 1–202.
    Chaudhry AA, Bhatti MN. 1992. Biology of grey francolin (Francolinus pondicerianus) in the central Punjab plains. Abstract Proc. 12th Pakistan Congress of Zoology, Lahore, pp 161–162.
    Clark A. 1901. Sport in the Low-country of Ceylon. Tisara Prakasakayo, Dehiwela.
    del Hoyo J, Elliott A, Sargatal J (eds). 1994. Handbook of the Birds of the World. Volume 2. New World Vultures to Guineafowl. Lynx Edicions, Barcelona, Spain, pp 412–567.
    Faruqui SA, Bump G, Nanda PC, Christensen GC. 1960. A study of the seasonal food of black francolin {Francolinus francolinus (Linnaeus)}, the gray francolin {Francolinus pondicerianus (Gmelin)} and the common sandgrouse {Pterocles exustus (Temminck)} in India and Pakistan. J Bombay Nat Hist Soc, 57: 354–361.
    Gould J. 1966. Birds of Europe. Methuen, London, pp 49–50.
    Helders S. 2007. Gujar Khan World Gazetteer. . Accessed on 5 September 2011
    Islam K. 1999. Erckel's francolin (Francolinus erckelii), black francolin (Francolinus francolinus), and grey francolin (Francolinus pondicerianus). In: Poole A, Gill F (eds) The Birds of North America. The Birds of North America, Inc., Philadelphia, pp 394–396.
    IUCN. 2011. IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2011.1. . Accessed on 05 September 2011.
    Javed S, Qureshi Q, Rahmani AR. 1999. Conservation status and distribution of swamp francolin in India. J Bombay Nat Hist Soc, 96: 16–23.
    Johnsgard PA. 1973. Grouse and quails of North America. University of Nebraska, Lincoln, p 553.
    Kaiser W. 1998. Autumn winter habitat used by radio-tagged grey partridge (perdix perdix) in north Bavaria. Game Wildlife Sci, 15: 471–479.
    Kamal S. 2000. Population status, distribution pattern and habitat of grey partridge (Francolinus pondicerianus) in district Mardan. Dissertations, Pakistan Forest Institute, Peshwar, p 50.
    Khan WA. 2010. Studies on the comparative ecology of the south Persian black francolin, Francolinus francolinus henrici, and northern grey partridge, Francolinus pondicerianus interpositus, in Lal Suhanra National Park, Bahawalpur, Punjab, Pakistan. Dissertations, Pir Mehr Ali Shah Arid Agriculture University, Rawalpindi, p 218.
    Long JL. 1981. Introduced Birds of the World. David and Charles, London, p 89.
    Mahmood K, Ahmad T, Khan A, Mahmood A, Mahmood W. 1997. Some notes on avifauna of Mangla reservoir, AJK, Pakistan. Pak J Ornith, 1: 1–2.
    Mahmood S, Mahmood T, Rais M, Qureshi IZ, Nadeem MS. 2010. Comparative study on population and habitat of the grey and black francolins; Francolinus pondicerianus and Francolinus francolinus in Lehri Nature Park, Punjab, Pakistan. Podoces, 5(1): 42–53.
    Mann MA, Chaudhry AA. 2000. Francolins in irrigated forest plantations and sub-mountainous tract of the Punjab, Pakistan. Pak Veterin J, 20(3): 118–122.
    Mc Crow VP. 1982. Grey partridge habitat use and nesting biology in north central Iowa. Dissertations, Iowa State University, Ames, p 239.
    Mian A, Ghani UI. 2007. Macrobiota variation under human protection in desert ecosystem of Cholistan, Pakistan. J Human Ecol, 21 (3): 163–172.
    Mian A, Wajid MM. 1994. Food of grey francolin (Francolinus pondicerianus) in Layyah District (southern Punjab, Pakistan). Pakistan J Zool, 26: 185–187.
    Mian A. 1995. Grey partridge demands intensive study. WPA News, 1: 2.
    Nizami MI, Shafiq M, Rashid A, Aslam A. 2004. The soils and their agricultural development potential in Pothwar. WRRI and NARC, Islamabad, Pakistan, p 5–7.
    Qureshi JI. 1972. Notes on the useful feeding activities of birds in field areas of Lyallpur. Pakistan J Agr Sci, 9 (3): 35–40.
    Richard AF, Caroll JP, McGown JPK. 2002. Partridges, Quails, Francolins, Snowcocks, Guineafowl and Turkeys. Status survey and conservation. World Pheasant Associ. BirdLife International /SSC Partridges, Quails and Francolin specialist group. IUCN: The World Conservation Union. Gland, Switzerland, p 58.
    Roberts TJ. 1991. The Birds of Pakistan, Non-passeriformes. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 232–233.
    Shafiq MM. 2005. Wildlife Acts and Rules of Pakistan. unpublished report. Pakistan Forest Institute, Peshawar, p 297.
    Sharma IK. 1983. The grey partridge (Francolinus pondicerianus) in the Rajasthan desert. Ann Arid Zone, 22: 117–120.
    Ullah H. 1991. Studies on the biology, habitat, distribution pattern, and food of the grey partridge (Francolinus pondicerianus) in tehsil, Faisalabad. Dissertations, University of Agriculture, Faisalabad, p 182.
    Waite HW. 1948. The birds of the Punjab salt range (Pakistan). J Bombay Nat Hist Soc, 48(1): 97–117.
    Wijeyamohan S, Vandercone R, Santiapillai C. 2003. Observations on the grey francolin (Francolinus pondicerianus) in the vicinity of Giant's Tank, Sri Lanka. PQF News. 19: 11–14.
    Yeatter RE. 1934. The Hungarian Partridge in Great Lakes Region. University of Michigan Press, Michigan, p 92.
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(36)

    1. Hanlin Yan, Longwu Wang, Laikun Ma, et al. Cuckoo eyes are an important identification cue for the Oriental reed warbler host. International Journal for Parasitology: Parasites and Wildlife, 2025, 26: 101038. DOI:10.1016/j.ijppaw.2025.101038
    2. Jiaojiao Wang, Laikun Ma, Taijun Zuo, et al. Distress calls in common cuckoo nestlings: targeting hosts or predators?. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 2025, 79(2) DOI:10.1007/s00265-025-03568-6
    3. Hanlin Yan, Longwu Wang, Wei Liang. Common cuckoo eggs are more resistant to puncture by the host. International Journal for Parasitology: Parasites and Wildlife, 2024, 25: 101003. DOI:10.1016/j.ijppaw.2024.101003
    4. Laikun Ma, Wei Liu, Peng Pan, et al. Oriental reed warblers retain strong egg recognition abilities during the nestling stage. Ecology and Evolution, 2024, 14(2) DOI:10.1002/ece3.11063
    5. Piotr Tryjanowski, Artur Golawski, Łukasz Jankowiak, et al. Reactions of wintering passerines to male calls of the European cuckoo Cuculus canorus. Scientific Reports, 2024, 14(1) DOI:10.1038/s41598-024-64270-7
    6. Bo Zhou, Wei Liang. Seasonal increase in nest defense, but not egg rejection, in a cuckoo host. Avian Research, 2024, 15: 100154. DOI:10.1016/j.avrs.2023.100154
    7. Hanlin Yan, Huahua Zhao, Haixia Luo, et al. Oriental Reed Warblers do not abandon Common Cuckoo chicks during prolonged nestling periods. Avian Research, 2024, 15: 100190. DOI:10.1016/j.avrs.2024.100190
    8. Dongyang Li, Yimei Zhang, Canwei Xia. Bird Interspecific Brood Parasitism Record in the Third Century. Birds, 2024, 5(4): 648. DOI:10.3390/birds5040044
    9. Ayumi Mizuno, Malgorzata Lagisz, Pietro Pollo, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of eyespot anti-predator mechanisms. eLife, 2024, 13 DOI:10.7554/eLife.96338.3
    10. Alfréd Trnka, Laikun Ma, Hanlin Yan, et al. Defense behavior of two closely related but geographically distant host species against cuckoo parasitism: A next test for the parallel coevolution. Ecology and Evolution, 2023, 13(6) DOI:10.1002/ece3.10175
    11. Csaba Moskát, Márk E. Hauber. On the sparrowhawk-like calls of female common cuckoos: testing for heterospecific vocal mimicry in a conspecific functional context. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 2023, 77(10) DOI:10.1007/s00265-023-03388-6
    12. Longwu Wang, Huahua Zhao, Haixia Luo, et al. Importance of cooperation: How host nest defenses effectively prevent brood parasitism from the cuckoos. iScience, 2023, 26(4): 106458. DOI:10.1016/j.isci.2023.106458
    13. Mairenn C. Attwood, Jess Lund, Chima J. Nwaogu, et al. Aggressive hosts are undeterred by a cuckoo's hawk mimicry, but probably make good foster parents. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2023, 290(1990) DOI:10.1098/rspb.2022.1506
    14. Angela Moreras, Jere Tolvanen, Risto Tornberg, et al. Breeding near heterospecifics as a defence against brood parasites: can redstarts lower probability of cuckoo parasitism using neighbours?. Oecologia, 2022, 199(4): 871. DOI:10.1007/s00442-022-05242-4
    15. Sahil Gupta, Avinash Agrawal, Kanan Saxena. Defense Strategies in Birds of Charadriidae Family. Proceedings of the Zoological Society, 2022, 75(4): 395. DOI:10.1007/s12595-022-00453-2
    16. Laikun Ma, Jianping Liu, Canchao Yang, et al. Egg mimicry and host selection by common cuckoos among four sympatric host species breeding within a reed habitat. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2022, 135(2): 407. DOI:10.1093/biolinnean/blab151
    17. Jiaojiao Wang, Laikun Ma, Xiangyang Chen, et al. Female Cuckoo Calls Deceive Their Hosts by Evoking Nest-Leaving Behavior: Variation under Different Levels of Parasitism. Animals, 2022, 12(15): 1990. DOI:10.3390/ani12151990
    18. Attila Marton, Attila Fülöp, Miklós Bán, et al. Female common cuckoo calls dampen the mobbing intensity of great reed warbler hosts. Ethology, 2021, 127(3): 286. DOI:10.1111/eth.13126
    19. Jun-Seo Go, Jin-Won Lee, Jeong-Chil Yoo. Variations of Hawk Mimicry Traits in the Four Sympatric Cuculus Cuckoos. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 2021, 9 DOI:10.3389/fevo.2021.702263
    20. Jennifer E. York. The Evolution of Predator Resemblance in Avian Brood Parasites. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 2021, 9 DOI:10.3389/fevo.2021.725842
    21. Xingyi Jiang, Changjie Zhang, Jianping Liu, et al. Female cuckoo calls elicit vigilance and escape responses from wild free-range chickens. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 2021, 33(1): 37. DOI:10.1080/03949370.2020.1792557
    22. Changjie Zhang, Xingyi Jiang, Meng Li, et al. Female cuckoo calls elicit anti-predatory behavior in birds. Journal of Ethology, 2021, 39(3): 393. DOI:10.1007/s10164-021-00716-z
    23. Oldřich Mikulica, Alfréd Trnka. On the behaviour and vocalizations of female Common Cuckoos Cuculus canorus at the host nest. Bird Study, 2021, 68(3): 422. DOI:10.1080/00063657.2022.2053944
    24. Ma Laikun, Yang Canchao, Liang Wei. Nest-Site Choice and Breeding Success among Four Sympatric Species of Passerine Birds in a Reedbed-Dominated Wetland. Journal of Resources and Ecology, 2021, 12(1) DOI:10.5814/j.issn.1674-764x.2021.01.003
    25. Jiaojiao Wang, Laikun Ma, Xiangyang Chen, et al. Behavioral and Acoustic Responses of the Oriental Reed Warbler (Acrocephalus orientalis), at Egg and Nestling Stages, to the Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus). Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 2021, 9 DOI:10.3389/fevo.2021.705748
    26. Yanyi Wang, Miao Tian, Jingpeng Liu, et al. Testing the Interspecific Function of Female Common Cuckoo “Bubbling” Call. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 2021, 9 DOI:10.3389/fevo.2021.725222
    27. Laikun Ma, Wei Liang. Egg rejection and egg recognition mechanisms in Oriental Reed Warblers. Avian Research, 2021, 12(1) DOI:10.1186/s40657-021-00283-4
    28. Lei Zhang, Jianping Liu, Haiwang Zhang, et al. Fight or flight: Geographic variation in antipredator defenses by cinereous tits. Global Ecology and Conservation, 2020, 24: e01207. DOI:10.1016/j.gecco.2020.e01207
    29. Jianping Liu, Canchao Yang, Jiangping Yu, et al. Egg recognition and brain size in a cuckoo host. Behavioural Processes, 2020, 180: 104223. DOI:10.1016/j.beproc.2020.104223
    30. Csaba Moskát, Márk E. Hauber, Jana Růžičková, et al. Female-female aggression and male responses to the two colour morphs of female common cuckoos. The Science of Nature, 2020, 107(4) DOI:10.1007/s00114-020-01680-3
    31. Longwu Wang, Canchao Yang, Gangbin He, et al. Cuckoos use host egg number to choose host nests for parasitism. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2020, 287(1928): 20200343. DOI:10.1098/rspb.2020.0343
    32. Chao Shen, Jiangping Yu, Hailin Lu, et al. Warblers perform less nest defense behavior and alarm calls to human intruders: A result of habituation. Global Ecology and Conservation, 2020, 23: e01187. DOI:10.1016/j.gecco.2020.e01187
    33. Jiaojiao Wang, Canchao Yang, James Hare. Specific responses of cuckoo hosts to different alarm signals according to breeding stage: a test of the offspring value hypothesis. Current Zoology, 2020, 66(6): 649. DOI:10.1093/cz/zoaa021
    34. Jiaojiao Wang, Qihong Li, Canchao Yang. Coevolution of acoustical communication between obligate avian brood parasites and their hosts. Avian Research, 2020, 11(1) DOI:10.1186/s40657-020-00229-2
    35. Jiangping Yu, Hailin Lu, Wei Sun, et al. Heterospecific alarm-call recognition in two warbler hosts of common cuckoos. Animal Cognition, 2019, 22(6): 1149. DOI:10.1007/s10071-019-01307-9
    36. Shelby L. Lawson, Janice K. Enos, Nicholas D. Antonson, et al. Advances in the Study of Behavior, DOI:10.1016/bs.asb.2021.03.002

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(3)  /  Tables(5)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (5779) PDF downloads (4089) Cited by(36)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return