Luchang Zhang, Xingjian Ma, Zhiyu Chen, Chunying Wang, Zicheng Liu, Xiang Li, Xiaoying Xing. 2023: Negative effects of artificial nest boxes on birds: A review. Avian Research, 14(1): 100101. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2023.100101
Citation: Luchang Zhang, Xingjian Ma, Zhiyu Chen, Chunying Wang, Zicheng Liu, Xiang Li, Xiaoying Xing. 2023: Negative effects of artificial nest boxes on birds: A review. Avian Research, 14(1): 100101. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2023.100101

Negative effects of artificial nest boxes on birds: A review

Funds: This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 32170485, 31501867), and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (Grant No. 2572022BE02)
More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    College of Wildlife and Protected Area, Northeast Forestry University, Harbin, 150040, China. E-mail address: ab71588@163.com (X. Xing)

  • Received Date: 11 Dec 2022
  • Rev Recd Date: 12 Apr 2023
  • Accepted Date: 12 Apr 2023
  • Available Online: 14 Jun 2023
  • Publish Date: 25 Apr 2023
  • Artificial nest boxes are placed to attract birds to nest and breed in a specific location, and they are widely used in avian ecology research and in the attraction of insectivorous birds. There is evidence that artificial nest boxes can adversely affect breeding fitness but no great focus has been placed on this issue by researchers. Therefore, we retrieved 321 research papers regarding artificial nest boxes published from 2003 to 2022 and used the ‘Biblioshiny’ program to extract and integrate keywords; we then summarized the adverse effects of artificial nest boxes on avian breeding success. The studies highlighted many drawbacks and misuses in the designing and placement of nest boxes; furthermore, bird attraction was decreased by their inappropriate selection, thus reducing breeding success. Regarding nest box production, there were shortcomings in the construction material, color, smell, and structural design of the boxes used. Nest boxes were also placed at inappropriate densities, locations, orientations, heights, and managed incorrectly. Finally, we propose suggestions for more efficient and safer artificial nest boxes for future use in avian ecology research and bird conservation.

  • Communication between individuals is important for attracting mates, advertising danger or food, defending territories, or maintaining contact (Hall, 2004; Sandoval et al., 2013; Tobergte and Curtis, 2013). In particular, communication appears to be a key component of territory and resource defense (Bradbury and Vehrencamp, 1998; Catchpole and Slater, 2008), because it allows for individuals to signal that a particular area or resource has an owner and may reduce future physical interactions between individuals. Physical interactions are energetically expensive and therefore need to be prevented (Hall, 2004; Fedy and Stutchbury, 2005). Territorial behavior varies temporally (Tinbergen, 1936; Nice, 1941; Stamps, 1994; Giuggioli et al., 2011); whereas some species defend territories or resources for short periods (Nice, 1941; Osborne and Bourne, 1977; Woltmann and Sherry, 2011), other species defend territories and resources year-round (Wikelski et al., 2000; Fedy and Stutchbury, 2005). Territorial behavior intensity may also change over time (Bodmer, 1990; Wikelski et al., 2000; Fedy and Stutchbury, 2005; Sosa-López et al., 2017) due to changes in the abundance of resources, breeding stage (i.e., non-reproductive vs. reproductive periods), hormone levels, and pair bond strength (Skutch, 1969; Møller, 1990; Wikelski et al., 2000; Fedy and Stutchbury, 2005). For example, territorial defense is negatively correlated with food abundance since a display of strong territorial behavior when resources are abundant will produce an unnecessary energy expenditure (Carpenter et al., 1983; Burker and Nol, 1998; Duca et al., 2006). Territorial behavior may intensify for small periods of time when individuals benefit from strong territorial display, as during the pre-breeding season when individuals fight over territorial limits (Gill et al., 2007; Sosa-López et al., 2017) or during the breeding season where individuals guard their mates to avoid extra-pair copulations or pair usurpation (Møller, 1990; MacDougall-Shackleton and Robertson, 1995; Juárez et al., 2020).

    To avoid unnecessary energy expenditures in territorial defense, many species (e.g., insects, amphibians, birds, and mammals) have developed the capability to distinguish between different territory intruder types using characteristics of territorial signals (e.g., vocal or visual signals) or through familiarity with the neighbors (Fisher, 1954; Temeles, 1994; Lovell and Lein, 2004). In birds, where vocal communication is primarily used for territory defense (Stamps, 1994; Giuggioli et al., 2011), it is expected that those species might use distinctive characteristics in frequency, duration, intensity, or complexity of acoustic signals to differentiate between intruders and respond to the threat of each intruder accordingly (Fisher, 1954; Temeles, 1994; Lovell and Lein, 2004). This has been found for Meadow Pipits (Anthus pratensis), where intruders are recognized using the characteristics of the introductory motif of the song, which varies in duration between individuals (Elfström, 1990). Additionally, Field Sparrow (Spizella pusilla) territorial males can distinguish between intruders based on the frequency characteristics of the songs (Nelson, 1989). Finally, Sylvia warblers also recognize threats for territory usurpation (from conspecific males) using the frequency and duration of the songs (Matyjasiak, 2005).

    Territorial individuals may also use the familiarity (daily interactions and habituation) with the intruder to respond according to their identity; because territorial individuals will have daily interactions with neighboring individuals but not with non-neighbors. For example, territorial birds share boundaries with other individuals and reduce the intensity and aggressiveness of the response to neighbors to reduce energetic expenditures in a phenomenon known as the "dear enemy effect" (Fisher, 1954; Lovell and Lein, 2004). In this case intruder recognition may be based on the familiarity with the location of neighboring individual and not with the acoustic characteristics of the signal (Falls and Brooks, 1975; Stoddard et al., 1991). This has been demonstrated in playback experiments with White-throated Sparrows (Zonotrichia albicollis) and Song Sparrows (Melospiza melodia), where the neighbor songs were reproduced from the opposite side of the natural territory boundaries, and responses toward songs reproduced on the opposite side of territory boundaries were of higher intensity and aggressiveness compared to responses towards songs reproduced from the existing side of the territory boundaries (Falls and Brooks, 1975; Stoddard et al., 1991).

    Examining how mechanisms of intruder recognition interact is key to our understanding of territorial establishment and defense. In this study our main objective was to analyze whether a year-round territorial bird species from the Neotropics discriminates between intruders using duet characteristics (i.e., duets' frequency and duration) or familiarity with potential intruders (i.e., neighbors and non-neighbors). We studied White-eared Ground-sparrows (Melozone leucotis), because this sparrow uses duets exclusively for territory defenses year-round (Sandoval et al., 2016, 2018; Juárez et al., 2020). Recognizing intruders using duet characteristics or familiarity will be advantageous for territory defense (primary functions of duets), especially in year-round territorial species, and allow pairs to avoid unnecessary energy expenditure against less threatening intruders (Fisher, 1954; Lovell and Lein, 2004). We tested two hypotheses on the use of duets in this species to determine the mechanisms used for intruder recognition. First, White-eared Ground-sparrow territorial pairs assess and distinguish the threat of intruders based on familiarity (i.e., neighbors vs. non-neighbors). We predict a less aggressive response by pairs towards the duets of neighboring pairs compared to non-neighboring pairs. In this scenario neighbors have already established their territories and through previous interactions with neighbors, both pairs recognize each other even when their duets are structurally different. The pairs will exhibit a reduced response towards their neighbors' duets and will save energy by avoiding unnecessary territorial disputes, as predicted for the dear enemy effect (Fisher, 1954; Lovell and Lein, 2004). If familiarity is not used to recognize neighbors in territory defense, we consequently predict the same level of aggressiveness towards neighbor and non-neighbor duets independently of duet characteristics. Second, White-eared Ground-sparrow territorial pairs assess and distinguish the threat of intruders based on duet characteristics (e.g., duration and frequency). We predict a more aggressive response towards duets that have more similar duration and frequency characteristics with duets of the territorial pair; these responses will occur independently from neighbors or non-neighbor status, as has been shown for Medium Ground-finches (Geospiza fortis), where territorial males respond stronger to songs from their own population (Podos, 2007). If duet characteristics are not used to recognize intruders, however, we predict the same level of aggression towards pairs with different degrees of duet distinctiveness.

    Contrary to the majority of species that produce duets using male and female solo songs (Wiley and Wiley, 1977; Molles and Vehrencamp, 2001), White-eared Ground-sparrows produce duets with a vocalization that is spectrotemporally different from male solo songs and used only for duetting (Sandoval et al., 2016). This probably originates from the fact that females do not sing solo songs in this species (Sandoval et al., 2016). Consequently, females contribute to territory and pair defense by duetting (Sandoval et al., 2018; Méndez and Sandoval, 2021). Both sexes initiate duets with equal frequency and produce polyphonal duets (duets that overlap in time and frequency; Fig. 1; Sandoval et al., 2013, 2016). Duets in this species have three sections: the introduction, started by one individual (male or female) which is composed of one to six elements; the middle part, that is composed of noisy broadband elements produced by the two individuals (the second individual starts to vocalize in this section); and the final part, produced by the second individual with distinctive elements that have a lower maximum frequency compared to the middle part (Fig. 1; Sandoval et al., 2016; Méndez and Sandoval, 2021). Previous research with White-eared Ground-sparrow has demonstrated that duets vary in frequency and duration characteristics among pairs by 11% based on discriminant function analysis (N ​= ​36; Sandoval et al., 2016).

    Figure  1.  Three examples of White-eared Ground-sparrow (Melozone leucotis) polyphonal duets used in playback experiments from the three populations studied, from April 30th to June 12th, 2018. Populations were located at: Getsemaní, Heredia province (10°01ʹ N, 84°05ʹ W, 1350 ​m); Universidad de Costa Rica campus, San José province (09°56ʹ N, 84°05ʹ W, 1200 ​m); Jardín Botánico Lankester, Cartago province (9°50ʹ N, 83°53ʹ W, 1370 ​m). Darker green areas represent areas at higher elevations. Yellow area represents the White-eared Ground-sparrow's distribution in Costa Rica. Broken line represents the introductory part of the duet. Black solid line represents the middle part of the duet. Gray solid line represents the final part of the duet.

    We conducted playback experiments in three populations of White-eared Ground-sparrows in the Central Valley, Costa Rica during the breeding season, from April 30th to June 12th, 2018. Populations were located at: (1) Universidad de Costa Rica campus, San José province (09°56ʹ N, 84°05ʹ W, 1200 ​m), (2) Getsemaní, Heredia province (10°01ʹ N, 84°05ʹ W, 1350 ​m), and (3) Jardín Botánico Lankester, Cartago province (9°50ʹ N, 83°53ʹ W, 1370 ​m). The White-eared Ground-sparrow populations studied in this playback experiment are part of a long-term study with color-banded individuals (Sandoval et al., 2013, 2016), allowing us to ensure that the same pairs were not resampled on different days. From March to June 2018, each pair was recorded for one to two days using a solid-state recorder (Marantz PMD661, sampling rate of 44.1 ​kHz, accuracy of 24 bit, and file format WAVE) and a shotgun microphone (Sennheiser ME66/K6) using the focal recording method, when this species is most vocally active between 04:55 and 06:00 ​h (Sandoval et al., 2016). We collected two to 15 duets from each pair. We quantified the spectrotemporal characteristics (minimum and maximum frequency, frequency of maximum amplitude, frequency range in Hz, duration in seconds) of duets using Raven Pro 1.6 (Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, NY, USA), with a frequency resolution of 188 ​Hz and a temporal resolution of 5.8 ​ms in a Hann window of 256 ​kHz sampling and 50% overlap. We obtained the measurements values using a visual assessment of the spectrogram, wave and power spectrum windows following the technique suggested for other authors previously (Podos, 2001). We used the spectrogram window to identify duets, the wave spectrum to measure time and power spectrum to measure frequencies.

    We used complete White-eared Ground-sparrow duets (Sandoval et al., 2016) recorded in March and April 2018 with the same equipment and focal recording method described above to create our playbacks. All duets used for playback stimuli came from different pairs, and all duets used for playback had a high signal-to-noise ratio and no overlap with other sounds. In several instances we had two or more duets from a given pair. For these cases, we assigned each duet a number from 1 to n, and then randomly selected a duet for a playback using the random number generator in Excel. We filtered all sounds below 5 ​kHz and above 11.5 ​kHz using the FFT filter in Adobe Audition 1.0 (Adobe System Incorporated, CA, USA). We standardized amplitude by normalizing duets to −1 ​dB with the same software. The filtered and normalized duets were repeated four times per minute following the rate used in previous territory studies for this species (Sandoval et al., 2013; Méndez and Sandoval, 2021). We created four stimuli: (1) neighbor duet: a duet from a pair that shares a border with the pair receiving the stimulus, (2) non-neighbor duet from the same population: a duet from a pair at least two territories apart with the pair receiving the stimulus, (3) non-neighbor duet from another population (minimum straight line distance between population was 13 ​km), and (4) control duet from Cabanis's Wren (Cantorchilus modestus). We used Cabani's wren for the control duet because they shars territorial areas with White-eared Ground-sparrows but have different diets and ecological requirements from White-eared Ground-sparrows. Each stimulus consisted of 2 ​min of playback followed by 5 ​min of silence. The four stimuli were presented in a consecutive sequence for a total of 28 ​min of experiment per pair, following previous playback experiment designs (e.g., Bolton, 2007; Geberzahn et al., 2009; Ripmeester et al., 2010; Sandoval et al., 2013; Sosa-López et al., 2016). The order of playback stimulus presentation to each territorial pair varied systematically. We presented each stimulus in each position (i.e., first, second, third, and fourth position) the same number of times within a population. The majority of pairs (71%) that approached the speaker returned to their original activities before the 2 ​min of stimulus ended, and no birds were observed within 3 ​m of the speaker at the end of the 5 ​min of silence. As in previous playback experiments for this ground-sparrow species (Sandoval et al., 2013, 2018), we broadcasted playbacks using an active loudspeaker (Anchor Audio Minivox, frequency response: 100–15, 000 ​Hz) and a portable audio player (iPod nano, Apple, CA, USA). To avoid any effect of differences in stimulus volume, we played back all stimulus at a constant volume of 80 ​dB SPL measured at 1 ​m distance to the loudspeakers using a digital sound level meter (Sper Scientific Digital Mini Sound Meter NIB – 850014), with weighting A and a fast response setup, as has been conducted in other playback experiments.

    The experiments were conducted on 39 pairs in total: 16 from the University of Costa Rica campus, 14 from Getsemaní, and 9 from Lankester Botanical Garden. We mounted the loudspeaker 1.5 ​m high on a pole 5 ​m inside of the boundary shared by pairs for each experimental playback session. Of the 39 pairs examined, 35 shared borders with only one neighbor, while the other four pairs shared borders with two neighbors on opposites side of the territory. The approach used in this study followed the approach and distances reported from other experiments that tested intruder responses for other bird species (Wiley and Wiley, 1977; Temeles, 1994; Lovell and Lein, 2004; Dingle and Salbbekoorn, 2018). Given that territories are not temporally static (Hixon et al., 1983; Møller, 1990; Butchart et al., 1999), we located the speaker inside the territorial pair to ensure that the stimulus was perceived as a territorial intrusion (Wiley and Wiley, 1977; Lovell and Lein, 2004). Additionally, with this approach we were confident that the pair could hear the stimulus from this position because the territory average of the White-eared Ground-sparrows is 0.17 ​ha (SE ​± ​0.03), which is approximately 42 ​× ​42 ​m in length (Juárez et al., 2020). Additionally, our single speaker experiment followed the design of previous playback experiments for this species (Sandoval et al., 2013, 2018) that showed aggressive responses of territorial pairs to duet stimuli, with both members of each pair producing duets near each other. Orange flagging tape (length: 10 ​cm) was positioned 3 ​m on both sides of the loudspeaker. These markings served as a spatial reference for behavioral responses during the trials and allowed for more accurate measurements of the time that took it to approach the speaker and total time spent near the speaker. This is especially important given how dense the vegetation (thicket habitats and coffee plantations; Sandoval and Mennill, 2012) is that this species inhabits. The observer was located 8 ​m away from the speaker, motionless, to reduce the observer effect on the ground-sparrow responses. Experiments were conducted between 06:00 and 09:30 ​h. Of the 7 ​min conducted for each playback stimulus, the first 2 ​min were of playback; the following 3 ​min of silence plus the first 2 ​min were our observation period, where we measured the following five behavioral responses: (1) latency from the start of the playback to the subjects' first vocalization (in seconds); a value of 301 ​s was assigned for no vocalizations; (2) latency of approaching to within 3 ​m of the speaker, referred to as the marked area (in seconds), a value of 301 ​s was assigned for no approach; (3) duration within the marked area (in seconds), a value of 0 ​s was assigned for no presence at all; (4) total number of individuals approaching inside the marked area (vary from 0 to 2 individuals); and (5) total number of vocalizations. It was possible to make measurements 1 to 4 directly in the field using a timer and counting the number of individuals that approached. The number of vocalizations was the total number of vocalizations made during the first 5 ​min of the stimuli. As in previous playback studies (Koloff and Mennill, 2011; Sandoval et al., 2013, 2018; Wu et al., 2021), we used the protocol of replacement 'no responses' with the maximum time possible value plus 1 unit (301 ​s), this is a solution to include 'no response' as part of our analysis and have a more robust sample size. As the last 2 ​min were considered recovery time, we stopped recording the behavior of the pair, but in all cases, both pair members returned during this time or before to conduct their prior behavior and abandoned the 3 ​m area around the speaker.

    To analyze if intruder recognition occurs using duets characteristics (i.e., frequency and duration) or familiarity with potential intruders (i.e., neighbors and non-neighbors), we combined four of the five behavioral responses into multivariate responses through a principal component analysis of the correlation matrix without rotation (KMO test: latency of approach ​= ​0.70 ​s; latency of first vocalization ​= ​0.81 ​s; duration within the marked area ​= ​0.72 ​s; number of individuals ​= ​0.83; Bartlett's Test of Sphericity: χ2 ​= ​265.63, df ​= ​6, p ​ < ​0.001). This first principal component had an eigenvalue of 2.78 and explained 69.42% of the total variance in the original variables (Appendix Table S1, S2). We called this multivariate response "aggressive response" (here after) because the first principal coordinate had a negative relationship with the latency of approach (r ​= ​−0.55; meaning faster approximation to the speakers) and latency of first vocalization (r ​= ​−0.37; meaning that the vocalizations were initiated faster), and a positive relationship with the duration within the marked area (r ​= ​0.54; meaning longer durations closer to the speaker) and the number of individuals that approached the speaker (r ​= ​0.52; meaning more individuals approaching the speaker). Conversely, the number of vocalizations was analyzed independently, because the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin-Criterium (KMO) had a value of 0.38, indicating that the data was not useful for inclusion in a factor analysis like principal component analysis.

    Before conducting any statistical analyses, we tested whether the presentation order of stimuli produced differences in pair responses. If the successive stimuli presentation has an effect on the intensity of response, we expected to find more aggressive responses in each stimulus compared to the previous one, independently of the stimulus type used in each position. This will occur when the time to recover between stimuli is too short, resulting in pairs responding faster and quicker to each successive stimulus. Therefore, we analyzed pair responses (PC1 and number of vocalizations) with two general linear mixed-effect models (GLMM) with a negative binomial distribution and a logit link function. We used a negative binomial distribution because it is appropriate for skewed and zero-bounded variables (Mun, 2008). We included stimuli order as our independent variable and pair identity as a random effect. Because these analyses showed no effect of stimulus order (PC1: t95 ​= ​−0.14, p ​= ​0.89 and number of vocalizations: t95 ​= ​0.77, p ​= ​0.44), we did not included order as a variable in our statistical analysis. To analyze if intruder recognition is based on neighbor familiarity we conducted two models (GLMM with negative binomial distribution and a logit link function) to analyze the effect of duet stimulus (independent variable with four levels: neighbor, non-neighbor, non-neighbor from other population, and control) in the aggressive response of territorial pairs (PC1 and number of vocalizations as dependent variables), and pair identity as a fixed-effect factor to control for the effect that each pair received four stimuli.

    The first step to determine whether duet similarity is used to recognize intruders per territorial pair is to analyze how duet similarity varies between populations. For this, we analyzed the effect of distance on duet similarity using a Mantel test with 10, 000 permutations. For this test we used Euclidian distance in both matrices, the first matrix included the geographic distances between the center of the pairs' territories and the second matrix included duet similarity using duration and frequency values. We also conducted a MANOVA to compare duet similarity between populations. The purpose of this analysis was to provide another measurement of duet similarity, in case duet similarity is not influenced by geographic distance. Following these two analyses we conducted another two GLMMs with negative binomial distribution and a logit link function using duet similarity estimated under the Euclidian distance of the Mantel test as our independent continuous variable. We used PC1 and number of vocalizations as dependent variables in each GLMM, and pair identity as a fixed-effect factor. Principal component analysis was conducted with the program JMP (version 7.0; SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA). Bartlett's test of sphericity and KMO tests were conducted in R (R Core Team, 2021) with the package "psych" (Revelle, 2021). GLMMs were conducted in R with the package "MASS" (Venables and Ripley, 2002) and "nlme" (Pinheiro et al., 2021). Mantel test and MANOVA were conducted in PAST 4.05.

    Our results were based on 32 pairs. Seven pairs (2 out of 13 ​at Getsemaní and 5 out of 16 ​at the University of Costa Rica campus) did not respond to any of our stimuli. When we analyzed familiarity in intruder recognition, we found differences in the intensity of the aggressive response (PC1) and number of vocalizations that White-eared Ground-sparrows produced according to the type of stimulus received. White-eared Ground-sparrows showed the strongest aggression towards neighbor simulated duets (t93 ​= ​5.17, p ​ < ​0.001) with an effect of 0.87 (SD ​± ​0.17) higher than control stimulus (Fig. 2). The level of aggression response for non-neighbors (t93 ​= ​2.61, p ​= ​0.01) and non-neighbors from other population duets (t93 ​= ​2.02, p ​= ​0.04) was lower than responses to neighbors, but higher than response towards control stimuli, for an effect of 0.47 (SD ​± ​0.18) and 0.37 (SD ​± ​0.18) respectively (Fig. 2). Overall, the aggression of responses for control duets (t93 ​= ​1.27, p ​= ​0.21; effect of 0.19, SD ​± ​0.16; Fig. 2) was very low. When compared across treatments, territorial pairs only produced more vocalizations in response to the neighbor stimulus (t93 ​= ​4.88, p ​ < ​0.001), with an effect of 1.57 (SD ​± ​0.32), which was higher than the response to the control stimuli (Fig. 2). The number of vocalizations in response to non-neighbor (t93 ​= ​0.96, p ​= ​0.34), non-neighbors from other population (t93 ​= ​1.51, p ​= ​0.14), and control duets (t93 ​= ​0.39, p ​= ​0.70) were very low and comparable in effect values (0.31 ​± ​0.33, 0.50 ​± ​0.33, and 0.13 ​± ​0.33; Fig. 2).

    Figure  2.  Differences in the response of territorial White-eared Ground-sparrow (Melozone leucotis) pairs according to the type of stimulus received. Black dots are the multivariate response obtained with the first component of the Principal Component Analysis; higher effect values represent faster approach and faster production of first vocalization, and more time within the marked area and the number of individuals that approached the speaker. Gray squares are the number of vocalizations produced per territorial pairs and higher effect values mean more vocalizations. Whiskers are SD of effect. The control species was Cabanis's Wren (Cantorchilus modestus).

    Duet similarity is not related to distance (Mantel: r ​= ​0.10, p ​= ​0.10), but duets were more similar within than between populations (MANOVA: F10, 96 ​= ​2.11, p ​= ​0.04). We found no relationship between the intensity of the aggressive response (PC1) and duet similarity (t93 ​= ​−1.76, p ​= ​0.08) with a lower effect of −0.10 (SD ​± ​0.06; Fig. 3). The number of vocalizations, however, increased in response to more similar duets (t93 ​= ​−2.76, p ​= ​0.008; with an effect value of −0.31 (SD ​± ​0.11; Fig. 3).

    Figure  3.  Relationship between duet similarity (using Euclidian distance; lower values represent more similar duets) and aggressive response of territorial White-eared Ground-sparrow (Melozone leucotis) pairs. Black dots are the multivariate response obtained with the first component of the Principal Component Analysis; higher effect values represent faster approach and faster production of the first vocalizations, increased time spent within the marked area, and the number of individuals that approached the speaker. Gray squares represent the number of vocalizations produced per territorial pairs.

    Our results showed that White-eared Ground-sparrows used familiarity for intruder recognition rather than acoustic characteristics of duets (i.e., frequency and duration). The level of aggression towards neighbors, however, was the highest compared to aggression towards non-neighbors (from the same or another population), contrary to the predictions of the dear enemy effect hypothesis (Fisher, 1954; Temeles, 1994). According to this hypothesis, a more aggressive response to non-neighbors is expected, followed by neighbors, and finally the control, to reduce energy expenditure towards familiar neighbors, which have defined territory borders.

    The dear enemy effect has been demonstrated in Stripe-backed Wrens (Campylorhynchus nuchalis), another year-round territorial species that uses duets for territory defense. Stripe-backed Wrens exhibit more aggressive behaviors against non-neighbor duets than neighbor duets (Wiley and Wiley, 1977). In this case, the authors justified the aggressive response against non-neighbors as a behavior to defend the territory from possible intruders (Wiley and Wiley, 1977). We found the opposite pattern in White-eared Ground-sparrows. The reason for this observation may occur because neighbors are a higher threat than non-neighbors in terms of territorial theft or mate usurpation (Ferkin, 1988; Temeles, 1989, 1990; Sandoval et al., 2018). This type of response also occurs for Meadow Voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus), where the probability of encountering a non-neighbor male in their own territory is lower in comparison to encountering a neighbor. Therefore, males demonstrate more aggressive behavior towards neighbors than non-neighbors (Ferkin, 1988). A similar phenomenon may occur with White-eared Ground-sparrows that share territory borders with several pairs over-years (Juárez et al., 2020), however, the majority of White-eared Ground-sparrows observed in this study share territorial borders with only a single pair during each breeding season (pers. obs.). Therefore, the probability of encountering a non-neighbor pair inside the territory is relatively low, and fighting may be costly, especially if they are only passing through (Ferkin, 1988). Additionally, if a new pair tries to create a territory in an established neighborhood, this new pair will not only be a threat to a single pair, but to all pairs that share territory borders within the target area.

    White-eared Ground-sparrow pairs were able to discriminate between neighbor and non-neighbor duets from the same population with low pair distinctiveness (Sandoval et al., 2016), but unable to distinguish between different non-neighbors. This result suggests that White-eared Ground-sparrow use familiarity instead of the duet acoustic characteristics we measured in this study to discriminate potential competitors from non-competitors (Falls and Brooks, 1975; Stoddard et al., 1991). We cannot rule out, however, that White-eared Ground-sparrow territorial pairs are using other characteristics not analyzed in this study (e.g., duet energy entropy or duet coordination) to recognize non-neighbors from their own and other populations. This phenomenon is not rare among bird species (e.g., medium ground-finches, Moltoni's warbler: Sylvia moltonii, or Hypocnemis antbird) where individuals are capable of recognizing their own population or specific individuals during playbacks, despite the inability to separate songs based on song measurements (Tobias and Seddon, 2003; Podos, 2007; Brambilla et al., 2008).

    How common the observations reported in this study are among long-lived tropical bird species with year-round territories (Skutch, 1969; Stutchbury and Morton, 2001; Fedy and Stutchbury, 2005; Battiston et al., 2015) is poorly known, especially for duetting species. Given, that acoustic characteristics of duets are also used to explain other behavioral hypotheses aside from the dear-enemy effect (Hall, 2004, 2009; Dahlin and Benedict, 2014), including this variable in future experiments will help to understand the relationship between function and structure for duets. This approach will provide more support to the growing body of evidence on the multiple functions of duets for tropical bird species (Hall, 2004, 2009; Dahlin and Benedict, 2014). Territories in the tropical habitats are not only defended by males and pairs, but they are also defended by family or non-family groups, females alone, during the breeding, or outside of the breeding season (Skutch, 1969; Stutchbury and Morton, 2001; Fedy and Stutchbury, 2005). Therefore, to develop a better understanding of the territorial behavior related to the mechanism of intruder recognition, a broad understanding of the species interactions during territorial defense is necessary.

    White-eared Ground-sparrows used familiarity with intruders as the main mechanism for the recognition of threats. Contrary to the dear enemy effect hypothesis, territorial ground-sparrow pairs produced more aggressive responses against neighbors than non-neighbors. This behavior may be the result of the original habitat inhabited by this species, young successional vegetation in forest gaps, landslides, and rivers edges (Sandoval and Mennill, 2012), or because neighbor pairs may pose a risk for pair usurpation or extra-pair copulation (Sandoval et al., 2018). The isolation of habitats and reduced area (Schemske and Brokaw, 1981) for territory establishment make non-neighbors rare over-years. The characteristics of these habitats may increase fights for territory boundaries between neighbor's over-time to secure more area and access to resources, making neighboring pairs more threatening than non-neighboring pairs. The availability of White-eared Ground-sparrow habitat has increased over the last 500 years (approximately) following increased human settlements inside this species' distribution. These developments have likely increased the pairs' abundances and the probability of encountering non-neighbor pairs, yet territorial pairs still recognize neighbors as more dangerous for territory usurpation. On the other hand, the risk of losing a mate or reproduction opportunities with neighbor pair members, suggests that duets are also used by White-eared Ground-sparrows for mate guarding and pair-bonding, and not just for territorial defense.

    TH and LS designed the experiment. TH, KB, IG, and LS conducted the data collection. BG and LS analyzed data. TH, BG, and LS wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

    All applicable international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed. Our study was approval by the Investigation Commission of Escuela de Biología, Universidad de Costa Rica, according to the Act on Experiments on Animals (Disposition No. 289 from 2005). Our playback experiments were kept brief to minimize disturbance and impact of the tested subjects.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    We are thankful for the Universidad de Costa Rica and Jardín Botánico Lankester for allowing us to conduct the field work on their properties. LS is also thankful for the partial time support of the Escuela de Biología. We are thankful ​to ​the two anonymous reviewers and the editor for all the constructive comments in a previous draft of this manuscript, which help to improve it. ​Financial support was provided by the Vicerrectoría de Investigación Universidad de Costa Rica through the project numbers B9123, B9469, and C1085.

    Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avrs.2022.100032.

  • Amo, L., Galván, I., Tomás, G., Sanz, J.J., 2008. Predator odour recognition and avoidance in a songbird. Funct. Ecol. 22, 289-293.
    Anctil, A., Franke, A., Bêty, J., 2014. Heavy rainfall increases nestling mortality of an Arctic top predator: experimental evidence and long-term trend in Peregrine Falcons. Oecologia 174, 1033-1043.
    Ardia, D.R., Pérez, J.H., Clotfelter, E.D., 2006. Nest box orientation affects internal temperature and nest site selection by Tree Swallows. J. Field Ornithol. 77, 339-344.
    Ardia, D.R., Pérez, J.H., Clotfelter, E.D., 2010. Experimental cooling during incubation leads to reduced innate immunity and body condition in nestling Tree Swallows. Proc. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 277, 1881-1888.
    Barbaro, L., Battisti, A., 2011. Birds as predators of the pine processionary moth (Lepidoptera: Notodontidae). Biol. Control 56, 107-114.
    Beard, R.L., 1972. Lethal action of UV irradiation on insects. J. Econ. Entomol. 65, 650-654.
    Becker, D.J., Singh, D., Pan, Q., Montoure, J.D., Talbott, K.M., Wanamaker, S.M., et al., 2020. Artificial light at night amplifies seasonal relapse of haemosporidian parasites in a widespread songbird. Proc. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 287, 20201831.
    Bhardwaj, M., Dale, C.A., Ratcliffe, L.M., 2015. Aggressive behavior by Western Bluebirds (Sialia mexicana) varies with anthropogenic disturbance to breeding habitat. Wilson J. Ornithol. 127, 421-431.
    Blickley, J.L., Word, K.R., Krakauer, A.H., Phillips, J.L., Sells, S.N., Taff, C.C., et al., 2012. Experimental chronic noise is related to elevated fecal corticosteroid metabolites in lekking male Greater Sage-Grouse (Centrocercus urophasianus). PLoS One 7, e50462.
    Breistøl, A., Högstedt, G., Lislevand, T., 2015. Pied Flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca prefer ectoparasite-free nest sites when old nest material is present. Ornis Nor. 38, 9-13.
    Brittingham, M.C., Williams, L.M., 2000. Bat boxes as alternative roosts for displaced bat maternity colonies. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 28, 197-207.
    Broughton, R.K., Hebda, G., Maziarz, M., Smith, K.W., Smith, L., Hinsley, S.A., 2015. Nest-site competition between bumblebees (Bombidae), social wasps (Vespidae) and cavity-nesting birds in Britain and the Western Palearctic. Hous. Theor. Soc. 62, 427-437.
    Browne, S.J., 2010. Effect of nestbox construction and colour on the occupancy and breeding success of nesting tits Parus spp. Hous. Theor. Soc. 53, 187-192.
    Burhans, D.E., Thompson, F.R., 2006. Songbird abundance and parasitism differ between urban and rural shrublands. Ecol. Appl. 16, 394-405.
    Burke, D.M., Elliott, K., Moore, L., Dunford, W., Nol, E., Phillips, J., et al., 2004. Patterns of nest predation on artificial and natural nests in forests. Conserv. Biol. 18, 381-388.
    Burton, N.H.K., 2007. Intraspecific latitudinal variation in nest orientation among ground-nesting passerines: a study using published data. Condor 109, 441-446.
    Bush, S.E., Clayton, D.H., 2018. Anti-parasite behaviour of birds. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 373, 20170196.
    Cade, T.J., 1973. Sun-bathing as a thermoregulatory aid in birds. Condor 75, 106-108.
    Cao, C.L., 2007. Edge effect on avian distribution and reproductive success in fragmented secondary-forest. Master’s Thesis. Northeast Normal University, Changchun.
    Carstens, K.F., Kassanjee, R., Little, R.M., Ryan, P.G., Hockey, P., 2019. Breeding success and population growth of Southern Ground Hornbills Bucorvus leadbeateri in an area supplemented with nest-boxes. Bird Conserv. Int. 29, 627-643.
    Catry, I., Franco, A.M.A., Sutherland, W.J., 2011. Adapting conservation efforts to face climate change: modifying nest-site provisioning for lesser kestrels. Biol. Conserv. 144, 1111-1119.
    Chamberlain, D.E., Cannon, A.R., Toms, M.P., Leech, D.I., Hatchwell, B.J., Gaston, K.J., 2009. Avian productivity in urban landscapes: a review and meta-analysis. Ibis 151, 1-18.
    Chan, A.A.Y-H., Giraldo-Perez, P., Smith, S., Blumstein, D.T., 2010. Anthropogenic noise affects risk assessment and attention: the distracted prey hypothesis. Biol. Lett. 6, 458-461.
    Chen, T.Y., 1989. Preliminary report on the experiment of artificial nest box in forest farm to attract tits. Chin. J. Zool. 24(2), 12 (in Chinese).
    Chen, S.H., Ding, P., Fan, Z.Y., Zheng, G.M., 2002. Selectivity of birds on urban woodlots. Zool. Res. 23, 31-38.
    Chen, L., Yang, G., Zou, H.F., Ma, J.Z., 2019. Study and design of artificial tree-cavity from the perspective of utility. Sci. Silv. Sin. 55, 141-148.
    Choi, C-Y., Nam, H-Y., Lee, E-J., Chung, O-S., Park, Y-S., Lee, J-K., et al., 2007. Nest box preference by secondary cavity-nesting birds in forested environments. J. Ecol. Field Biol. 30, 49-56.
    Christe, P., Richner, H., Oppliger, A., 1996. Of great tits and fleas: sleep baby sleep. Anim. Behav. 52, 1087-1092.
    Clark, L.B., Shave, M.E., Hannay, M.B., Lindell, C.A., 2020. Nest box entrance hole size influences prey delivery success by American Kestrels. J. Raptor Res. 54, 303-310.
    Cook, D.B., 1947. A useful nestbox design. J. Wildl. Manag. 11, 185-188.
    Curtin, C.G., 2002. Livestock grazing, rest, and restoration in arid landscapes. Conserv. Biol. 16, 840-842.
    Custer, C.M., Custer, T.W., Dummer, P.M., Munney, K.L., 2003. Exposure and effects of chemical contaminants on tree swallows nesting along the Housatonic River, Berkshire County, Massachusetts, USA, 1998-2000. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 22, 1605-1621.
    Czeszczewik, D., 2004. Breeding success and timing of the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca nesting in natural holes and nest-boxes in the Bialowieza Forest, Poland. Acta Ornithol. 39, 15-20.
    Dawson, R.D., Lawrie, C.C., O'Brien, E.L., 2005. The importance of microclimate variation in determining size, growth and survival of avian offspring: experimental evidence from a cavity nesting passerine. Oecologia 144, 499-507.
    Demeyrier, V., Lambrechts, M.M., Perret, P., Grégoire, A., 2016. Experimental demonstration of an ecological trap for a wild bird in a human-transformed environment. Anim. Behav. 118, 181-190.
    Dhondt, A.A., 2012. Interspecific Competition in Birds. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
    Ding, G.Q., Guan, J.D., Fang, L.J., 1999. Biological problems that should be paid attention to in increasing forest bird attraction rate. J. Liaoning For. Sci. Tech. 2, 28-29.
    Dominoni, D.M., Goymann, W., Helm, B., Partecke, J., 2013. Urban-like night illumination reduces melatonin release in European Blackbirds (Turdus merula): implications of city life for biological time-keeping of songbirds. Front. Zool. 10, 60.
    Du Feu, C.R.D., 1992. How tits avoid flea infestation at nest sites. Ringing Migr. 13, 120-121.
    Duckworth, R.A., Hallinger, K.K., Hall, N., Potticary, A.L., 2017. Switch to a novel breeding resource influences coexistence of two passerine birds. Front. Ecol. Evol. 5, 72.
    Durant, S.E., Hopkins, W.A., Hepp, G.R., Walters, J.R., 2013. Ecological, evolutionary, and conservation implications of incubation temperature-dependent phenotypes in birds. Biol. Rev. 88, 499-509.
    Eggers, S., Griesser, M., Nystrand, M., Ekman, J., 2006. Predation risk induces changes in nest-site selection and clutch size in the Siberian Jay. Proc. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 273, 701-706.
    Ekner, A., Tryjanowski, P., 2008. Do small hole nesting passerines detect cues left by a predator? A test on winter roosting sites. Acta Ornithol. 43, 107-111.
    Ellis, M.V., 2016. Influence of design on the microclimate in Nest boxes exposed to direct sunshine. Aust. Zool. 38, 95-101.
    Ellis, M.V., Rhind, S., 2021. Designing better nestboxes: double-walled and pale proves coolest under the sun. Pacific Conserv. Biol. 28, 444-454.
    Enemar, A., Sjöstrand, B., Andersson, G., von Proschwitz, T., 2004. The 37-year dynamics of a subalpine passerine bird community, with special emphasis on the influence of environmental temperature and Epirrita autumnata cycles. Ornis Svec. 14, 63-106.
    Evans, M.R., Lank, D.B., Boyd, W.S., Cooke, F., 2002. A comparison of the characteristics and fate of Barrow's Goldeneye and Bufflehead nests in nest boxes and natural cavities. Condor 104, 610-619.
    Evans, K.L., Chamberlain, D.E., Hatchwell, B.J., Gregory, R.D., Gaston, K.J., 2011. What makes an urban bird? Global Change Biol. 17, 32-44.
    Farner, D.S., 1964. The photoperiodic control of reproductive cycles in birds. Am. Sci. 52, 137-156.
    Fehérvári, P., Piross, I.S., Kotymán, L., Solt, S., Horváth, É., Palatitz, P., 2015. Species specific effect of nest-box cleaning on settlement selection decisions in an artificial colony system. Ornis Hung. 23, 66-76.
    Fuentes, D., Rubalcaba, J.G., Veiga, J.P., Polo, V., 2019. Long-term fitness consequences of breeding density in starling colonies: an observational approach. J. Ornithol. 160, 1035-1042.
    García-Navas, V., Arroyo, L., Sanz, J.J., Díaz, M., 2010. Effect of nestbox type on occupancy and breeding biology of Tree Sparrows Passer montanus in central Spain. Ibis 150, 356-364.
    Gates, J.E., Gysel, L.W., 1978. Avian nest dispersion and fledging success in field-forest ecotones. Ecology 59, 871-883.
    Goldingay, R.L., 2015. Temperature variation in nest boxes in eastern Australia. Aust. Mamm. 37, 225-233.
    Goldingay, R.L., Stevens, J.R., 2009. Use of artificial tree hollows by Australian birds and bats. Wildl. Res. 36, 81-97.
    Gomez, D., Grégoire, A., del Rey Granado, M., Bassoul, M., Degueldre, D., Perret, P., et al., 2014. The intensity threshold of colour vision in a passerine bird, the Blue Tit (Cyanistes caeruleus). J. Exp. Biol. 217, 3775-3778.
    Griffiths, S.R., Rowland, J.A., Briscoe, N.J., Lentini, P.E., Handasyde, K.A., Lumsden, L.F., et al., 2017. Surface reflectance drives nest box temperature profiles and thermal suitability for target wildlife. PLoS One 12, e0176951.
    Griffiths, S.R., Semmens, K., Watson, S.J., Jones, C.S., 2020. Installing chainsaw-carved hollows in medium-sized live trees increases rates of visitation by hollow-dependent fauna. Restor. Ecol. 28, 1225-1236.
    Grüebler, M.U., Widmer, S., Korner-Nievergelt, F., Naef-Daenzer, B., 2014. Temperature characteristics of winter roost-sites for birds and mammals: tree cavities and anthropogenic alternatives. Int. J. Biometeorol. 58, 629-637.
    Gwinner, E., Scheuerlein, A., 1998. Seasonal changes in day-light intensity as a potential zeitgeber of circannual rhythms in equatorial Stonechats. J. Ornithol. 139, 407-412.
    Hale, R., Swearer, S.E., 2016. Ecological traps: current evidence and future directions. Proc. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 283, 20152647.
    Halfwerk, W., Bot, S., Buikx, J., van der Velde, M., Komdeur, J., ten Cate, C., et al., 2011a. Low-frequency songs lose their potency in noisy urban conditions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108, 14549-14554.
    Halfwerk, W., Holleman, L.J.M., Lessells, C.M., Slabbekoorn, H., 2011b. Negative impact of traffic noise on avian reproductive success. J. Appl. Ecol. 48, 210-219.
    Halfwerk, W., Bot, S., Slabbekoorn, H., Williams, T., 2012. Male Great Tit song perch selection in response to noise-dependent female feedback. Funct. Ecol. 26, 1339-1347.
    Harper, M.J., McCarthy, M.A., van der Ree, R., 2005. The use of nest boxes in urban natural vegetation remnants by vertebrate fauna. Wildl. Res. 32, 509-516.
    He, M., Yang, M.S., Bei, Y.F., Yang, Y.J., Luo, X., Wang, Y., et al., 2020. Research progress on the impact of urbanization and urban green space on urban birds. Hubei Agric. Sci. 59, 11-15.
    Hebda, G., Wesołowski, T., 2012. Low flea loads in birds' nests in tree cavities. Ornis Fenn. 89, 139-144.
    Hebda, G.A., Kandziora, A., Mitrus, S., 2017. Decomposition of nest material in tree holes and nest-boxes occupied by European Starlings Sturnus vulgaris: an experimental study. Acta Ornithol. 52, 119-125.
    Heeb, P., Kölliker, M., Richner, H., 2000. Bird-ectoparasite interactions, nest humidity, and ectoparasite community structure. Ecology 81, 958-968.
    Hofstetter, S.H., Ritchison, G., 1998. The begging behavior of nestling Eastern Screech-Owls. Wilson Bull. 110, 86-92.
    Holmes, R.T., Schultz, J.C., 1988. Food availability for forest birds: effects of prey distribution and abundance on bird foraging. Can. J. Zool. 66, 720-728.
    Holveck, M.J., Doutrelant, C., Guerreiro, R., Perret, P., Gomez, D., Grégoire, A., 2010. Can eggs in a cavity be a female secondary sexual signal? Male nest visits and modelling of egg visual discrimination in Blue Tits. Biol. Lett. 6, 453-457.
    Holveck, M.J., Grégoire, A., Doutrelant, C., Lambrechts, M.M., 2019. Nest height is affected by lamppost lighting proximity in addition to nestbox size in urban Great Tits. J. Avian Biol. 50, e01798.
    Huhta, E., 2000. Artificial nest predation and abundance of birds along an urban gradient. Condor 102, 838-847.
    Hurlbert, A.H., Haskell, J.P., 2003. The effect of energy and seasonality on avian species richness and community composition. Am. Nat. 161, 83-97.
    Isaac, J.L., de Gabriel, J.L., Goodman, B.A., 2008. Microclimate of daytime den sites in a tropical possum: implications for the conservation of tropical arboreal marsupials. Anim. Conserv. 11, 281-287.
    Jaworski, T., Gryz, J., Krauze-Gryz, D., Plewa, R., Bystrowski, C., Dobosz, R., et al., 2022. My home is your home: nest boxes for birds and mammals provide habitats for diverse insect communities. Insect Conserv. Divers. 15, 461-469.
    Jiang, X.L., Wang, R.R., Li, Z.Q., Zhang, Z.Y., 2012. Breeding behavior of sympatric Silky Starlings and White-cheeked Starlings. Chin. J. Ecol. 31, 2011-2015.
    Jones, T.M., Durrant, J., Michaelides, E., Green, M.P., 2015. Melatonin: a possible link between the presence of artificial light at night and reductions in biological fitness. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 370, 20140122.
    Julliard, R., Mccleery, R.H., Clobert, J., Perrins, C.M., 1997. Phenotypic adjustment of clutch size due to nest predation in the Great Tit. Ecology 78, 394-404.
    Kaluthota, C.D., Rendall, D., 2017. Nest site selection and breeding biology of Western House Wrens (Troglodytes aedon parkmanii) using natural cavities in Western Canada. Can. J. Zool. 95, 505-514.
    Karlsson, J., Nilsson, S.G., 1977. The influence of nest-box area on clutch size in some hole-nesting passerines. Ibis 119, 207-211.
    Kempenaers, B., Verheyen, G.R., van den Broeck, M., Burke, T., van Broeckhoven, C., Dhondt, A., 1992. Extra-pair paternity results from female preference for high-quality males in the Blue Tit. Nature 357, 494-496.
    Kerth, G., Weissmann, K., König, B., 2001. Day roost selection in female Bechtein’s bats (Myotis bechteinii): a field experiment to determine the influence of roost temperature. Oecologia 126, 1-9.
    Kilgas, P., Tilgar, V., Mägi, M., Mänd, R., 2007. Physiological condition of incubating and brood rearing female Great Tits Parus major in two contrasting habitats. Acta Ornithol. 42, 129-136.
    Klein, Á., Nagy, T., Csörgő, T., Mátics, R., 2007. Exterior nest-boxes may negatively affect Barn Owl Tyto alba survival: an ecological trap. Bird Conserv. Int. 17, 273-281.
    Krams, R., Krama, T., Brūmelis, G., Elferts, D., Strode, L., Dauškane, I., et al., 2021. Ecological traps: evidence of a fitness cost in a cavity-nesting bird. Oecologia 196, 735-745.
    Krijgsveld, K.L., Visser, G.H., Daan, S., 2003. Foraging behavior and physiological changes in precocial quail chicks in response to low temperatures. Physiol. Behav. 79, 311-319.
    Kurucz, K., Purger, J.J., Batáry, P., 2021. Urbanization shapes bird communities and nest survival, but not their food quantity. Glob. Ecol. Conserv. 26, e01475.
    Lambrechts, M.M., Adriaensen, F., Ardia, D.R., Artemyev, A.V., Atiénzar, F., Bańbura, J., et al., 2010. The design of artificial nestboxes for the study of secondary hole-nesting birds: a review of methodological inconsistencies and potential biases. Acta Ornithol. 45, 1-26.
    Lambrechts, M.M., Charmantier, A., Demeyrier, V., Lucas, A., Perret, S., Abouladzé, M., et al., 2017. Nest design in a changing world: Great Tit Parus major nests from a Mediterranean city environment as a case study. Urban Ecosyst. 20, 1181-1190.
    Lan, F., Ma, X., Lu, J., Li, Y., Chai, R., Li, X., et al., 2021. Effects of urbanization on bird nesting: a review. Biodivers. Sci. 29, 1539-1553.
    Latif, Q.S., Heath, S.K., Rotenberry, J.T., 2012. How avian nest site selection responds to predation risk: testing an ‘adaptive peak hypothesis. J. Anim. Ecol. 81, 127-138.
    Lei, B.R., Green, J.A., Pichegru, L., 2014. Extreme microclimate conditions in artificial nests for Endangered African Penguins. Bird Conserv. Int. 24, 201-213.
    Leroux, S.L., McKellar, A.E., Flood, N.J., Paetkau, M.J., Bailey, J.M., Reudink, M.W., et al., 2018. The influence of weather and parental provisioning on fledging success depends on nest box type in a cavity-nesting passerine, the Mountain Bluebird (Sialia currucoides). Wilson J. Ornithol. 130, 708-715.
    Lesiński, G., 2000. Location of bird nests in vertical metal pipes in suburban built-up area of Warsaw. Acta Ornithol. 35, 211-214.
    Leveau, L.M., 2018. Urbanization, environmental stabilization and temporal persistence of bird species: a view from Latin America. PeerJ 6, e6056.
    Li, L.Y., 2015. Effects of changing nest site resources on birds community structure formation in the secondary forest. Master’s Thesis. Jilin Agricultural University, Changchun.
    Li, Z., Yang, L.Y., Liu, W., Deng, W.H., 2008. Effect of artificial nestboxes on the diversity of secondary cavity-nesting birds and the stability of breeding bird community. Biodiversity Sci.6, 601-606.
    Liang, L.M., 2018. Researches on pests control method by attracting bird using artificial nest boxes. Liaoning For. Sci. Tech. 2, 40-42.
    Liu, Y., 2005. Nesting Success of Great Tit Nesting in Nest-Boxes and the Use of Nest-Boxes. Master's Thesis. Northeast Normal University, Changchun.
    Liu, D.P., Li, G.D., 2020. A case of conspecific brood parasitism in the Scaly-sided Merganser (Mergus squamatus) in the changbai mountains, China. Chin. J. Zool. 55, 407-410.
    Lohr, B., Wright, T.F., Dooling, R.J., 2003. Detection and discrimination of natural calls in masking noise by birds: estimating the active space of a signal. Anim. Behav. 65, 763-777.
    Lourenço, S.I., Palmeirim, J.M., 2004. Influence of temperature in roost selection by Pipistrellus pygmaeus (Chiroptera): relevance for the design of bat boxes. Biol. Conserv. 119, 237-243.
    Lucass, C., Eens, M., Müller, W., 2016. When ambient noise impairs parent-offspring communication. Environ. Pollut. 212, 592-597.
    Mac Arthur, R.H., 1959. On the breeding distribution pattern of North American migrant birds. Auk 76, 318-325.
    Mainwaring, M.C., 2017. Causes and consequences of intraspecific variation in nesting behaviors: insights from Blue Tits and Great Tits. Front. Ecol. Evol. 5, 39.
    Mainwaring, M.C., Hartley, I.R., Lambrechts, M.M., Deeming, D.C., 2014. The design and function of birds. Nest. Ecol. Evol. 4, 3909-3928.
    Malek, I., Haim, A., 2019. Bright artificial light at night is associated with increased body mass, poor reproductive success and compromised disease tolerance in Australian Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Integr. Zool. 14, 589-603.
    Mänd, R., Tilgar, V., Lõhmus, A., Leivits, A., 2005. Providing nest boxes for hole-nesting birds − does habitat matter? Biodivers. Conserv. 14, 1823-1840.
    Mänd, R., Leivits, A., Leivits, M., Rodenhouse, N.L., 2009. Provision of nestboxes raises the breeding density of Great Tits Parus major equally in coniferous and deciduous woodland. Ibis 151, 487-492.
    Martin, T.E., 1987. Artificial nest experiments: effects of nest appearance and type of predator. Condor 89, 925-928.
    Martin, T.E., 1993a. Evolutionary determinants of clutch size in cavity-nesting birds: nest predation or limited breeding opportunities? Am. Nat. 142, 937-946.
    Martin, T.E., 1993b. Nest predation among vegetation layers and habitat types: revising the dogmas. Am. Nat. 141, 897-913.
    Martínez-Abraín, A., Jiménez, J., 2019. Dealing with growing forest insect pests: the role of top-down regulation. J. Appl. Ecol. 56, 2574-2576.
    Marzluff, J.M., 2017. A decadal review of urban ornithology and a prospectus for the future. Ibis 159, 1-13.
    Marzluff, J.M., Bowman, R., Donnelly, R., 2001. Avian Ecology and Conservation in an Urbanizing World. Springer, New York.
    Mazgajski, T.D., 2007a. Effect of old nest material in nestboxes on ectoparasite abundance and reproductive output in the European Starling Sturnus vulgaris. Pol. J. Ecol. 55, 377-385.
    Mazgajski, T.D., 2007b. Effect of old nest material on nest site selection and breeding parameters in secondary hole nesters-a review. Acta Ornithol. 42, 1-14.
    Maziarz, M., Broughton, R.K., Wesołowski, T., 2017. Microclimate in tree cavities and nest-boxes: implications for hole-nesting birds. For. Ecol. Manag. 389, 306-313.
    Mazuc, J., Bonneaud, C., Chastel, O., Sorci, G., 2010. Social environment affects female and egg testosterone levels in the House Sparrow (Passer domesticus). Ecol. Lett. 6, 1084-1090.
    McCleery, R., Clobert, J., Julliard, R., Perrins, C., 1996. Nest predation and delayed cost of reproduction in the Great Tit. J. Anim. Ecol. 65, 96-104.
    Mccomb, W.C., Noble, R.E., 1981. Microclimates of nest boxes and natural cavities in bottomland hardwoods. J. Wildl. Manag. 45, 284-289.
    Mcnabb, E.D., Greenwood, J., 2011. A powerful owl disperses into town and uses an artificial nest-box. Aust. Field Ornithol. 28, 65-75.
    Meyrom, K., Motro, Y., Leshem, Y., Aviel, S., Izhaki, I., Argyle, F., et al., 2009. Nest-box use by the Barn Owl Tyto alba in a biological pest control program in the Beit She’an Valley, Israel. Ardea 97, 463-467.
    Mingju, E., Wang, T., Wang, S., Gong, Y., Yu, J., Wang, L., et al., 2019. Old nest material functions as an informative cue in making nest-site selection decisions in the European Kestrel (Falco tinnunculus). Avian Res. 10, 43.
    Moeed, A., Dawson, D.G., 1979. Breeding of Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) in nest boxes of various types. N. Z. J. Zool 6, 613-618.
    Møller, A.P., Moller, A.P., 1989. Parasites, predators and nest boxes: facts and artefacts in nest box studies of birds? Oikos 56, 421-423.
    Møller, A., Nielsen, J., Garamszegi, L.Z., 2006. Song post exposure, song features, and predation risk. Behav. Ecol. 17, 155-163.
    Moore, T., de Tores, P., Fleming, P.A., 2010. Detecting, but not affecting, nest-box occupancy. Wildl. Res. 37, 240-248.
    Moyer, B.R., Wagenbach, G.E., 1995. Sunning by Black Noddies (Anous minutus) may kill Chewing Lice (Quadraceps hopkinsi). Auk 112, 1073-1077.
    Mulholland, T.I., Ferraro, D.M., Boland, K.C., Ivey, K.N., Le, M.L., LaRiccia, C.A., et al., 2018. Effects of experimental anthropogenic noise exposure on the reproductive success of secondary cavity nesting birds. Integr. Comp. Biol. 58, 967-976.
    Murphy, M.T., 1985. Nestling eastern kingbird growth: effects of initial size and ambient temperature. Ecology 66, 162-170.
    Newton, I., 2007. Population limitation in birds: the last 100 years. Br. Birds 100, 518-539.
    Nicolaus, M., Both, C., Ubels, R., Tinbergen, E., 2009. No experimental evidence for local competition in the nestling phase as a driving force for density-dependent avian clutch size. J. Anim. Ecol. 78, 828-838.
    Norris, A.R., Aitken, K.E., Martin, K., Pokorny, S., 2018. Nest boxes increase reproductive output for Tree Swallows in a forest grassland matrix in central British Columbia. PLoS One 13, e0204226.
    Nussear, K.E., Simandle, E.T., Tracy, R., 2000. Misconceptions about colour, infrared radiation, and energy exchange between animals and their environments. Herpetol. J. 10, 119-122.
    Orell, M., Ojanen, M., 1983. Breeding biology and population dynamics of the Willow Tit Parus montanus. Ann. Zool. Fenn. 20, 99-114.
    Ouyang, J.Q., de Jong, M., van Grunsven, R.H.A., Matson K.D., Haussmann, M.F., Meerlo, P., et al., 2017. Restless roosts: light pollution affects behavior, sleep, and physiology in a free-living songbird. Global Change Biol. 23, 4987-4994.
    Pacejka, A.J., Santana, E., Harper, R.G., Thompson, C.F., 1996. House Wrens Troglodytes aedon and nest-dwelling ectoparasites: mite population growth and feeding patterns. J. Avian Biol. 27, 273-278.
    Patricelli, G.L., Blickley, J.L., 2006. Avian communication in urban noise: causes and consequences of vocal adjustment. Auk 123, 639-649.
    Pérez, J.H., Ardia, D.R., Chad, E.K., Clotfelter, E.D., 2008. Experimental heating reveals nest temperature affects nestling condition in Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor). Biol. Lett. 4, 468-471.
    Petit, L.J., Petit, D.R., 1996. Factors governing habitat selection by prothonotary warblers: field tests of the Fretwell-Lucas models. Ecol. Monogr. 66, 367-387.
    Pijanowski, B.C., Farina, A., Gage, S.H., Dumyahn, S.L., Krause, B.L., 2011. What is soundscape ecology? An introduction and overview of an emerging new science. Landsc. Ecol. 26, 1213-1232.
    Podkowa, P., Malinowska, K., Surmacki, A., 2019. Light affects parental provisioning behaviour in a cavity-nesting Passerine. J. Avian Biol. 50, e02254.
    Puchala, P., 2004. Detrimental effects of larval Blow Flies (Protocalliphora azurea) on nestlings and breeding success of Tree Sparrows (Passer montanus). Can. J. Zool. 82, 1285-1290.
    Quinn, J.L., Whittingham, M.J., Butler, S.J., Cresswell, W., 2006. Noise, predation risk compensation and vigilance in the Chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. J. Avian Biol. 37, 601-608.
    Raap, T., Pinxten, R., Eens, M., 2015. Light pollution disrupts sleep in free-living animals. Sci. Rep. 5, 13557.
    Raap, T., Casasole, G., Costantini, D., Abdelgawad, H., Asar, D.H., Pinxten, R., et al., 2016a. Artificial light at night affects body mass but not oxidative status in free-living nestling songbirds: an experimental study. Sci. Rep. 6, 35626.
    Raap, T., Casasole, G., Pinxten, R., Eens, M., 2016b. Early life exposure to artificial light at night affects the physiological condition: an experimental study on the ecophysiology of free-living nestling songbirds. Environ. Pollut. 218, 909-914.
    Raap, T., Pinxten, R., Eens, M., 2016c. Artificial light at night disrupts sleep in female Great Tits (Parus major) during the nestling period, and is followed by a sleep rebound. Environ. Pollut. 215, 125-134.
    Radford, A.N., Du Plessis, M.A., 2003. The importance of rainfall to a cavity-nesting species. Ibis 145, 692-694.
    Rahn, H., Ackerman, R., Paganelli, C., 1977. Humidity in the avian nest and egg water loss during incubation. Physiol. Zool. 50, 269-283.
    Reid, J.M., Monaghan, P., Ruxton, G.D., 2000. Resource allocation between reproductive phases: the importance of thermal conditions in determining the cost of incubation. Proc. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 267, 37-41.
    Rendell, W.B., Verbeek, N.A.M., 1996. Are avian ectoparasites more numerous in nest boxes with old nest material? Can. J. Zool. 74, 1819-1825.
    Reynold, S.J., Davies, C.S., Elwell, E., Tasker, P.J., William, A., Sadler, J.P., et al., 2016. Does the urban gradient influence the composition and ectoparasite load of nests of an urban bird species? Avian Biol. Res. 9, 224-234.
    Rheindt, F.E., 2003. The impact of roads on birds: does song frequency play a role in determining susceptibility to noise pollution? J. Ornithol. 144, 295-306.
    Rhodes, B., O'Donnell, C., Jamieson, I., 2009. Microclimate of natural cavity nests and its implications for a threatened secondary-cavity-nesting passerine of New Zealand, the South Island saddleback. Condor 111, 462-469.
    Robertson, G.J., 1995. Factors affecting nest site selection and nesting success in the common eider Somateria mollissima. Ibis 137, 109-115.
    Hutto, R.R.L., 2006. A framework for understanding ecological traps and an evaluation of existing evidence. Ecology 87, 1075-1085.
    Robertson, B.A., Rehage, J.S., Sih, A., 2013. Ecological novelty and the emergence of evolutionary traps. Trends Ecol. Evol. 28, 552-560.
    Robinson, S.K., Holmes, R.T., 1982. Foraging behavior of forest birds: the relationships among search tactics, diet, and habitat structure. Ecology 63, 1918-1931.
    Rytkönen, S., Krams, I., 2003. Does foraging behaviour explain the poor breeding success of Great Tits Parus major in northern Europe? J. Avian Biol. 34, 288-297.
    Rytkönen, S., Orell, M., 2001. Great Tits, Parus major, lay too many eggs: experimental evidence in mid-boreal habitats. Oikos 93, 439-450.
    Salaberria, C., Celis, P., López-Rull, I., Gil, D., 2014. Effects of temperature and nest heat exposure on nestling growth, dehydration and survival in a Mediterranean hole-nesting passerine. Ibis 156, 265-275.
    Schroeder, J., Nakagawa, S., Cleasby, I.R., Burke, T., 2012. Passerine birds breeding under chronic noise experience reduced fitness. PLoS One 7, e39200.
    Schwartz, T., Genouville, A., Besnard, A., 2020. Increased microclimatic variation in artificial nests does not create ecological traps for a secondary cavity breeder, the European roller. Ecol. Evol. 10, 13649-13663.
    Shi, Y.F., Liu, J.C., 2018. Study on bamboo artificial bird's nest design. Agric. Tec. (Santiago) 38, 165.
    Skwarska, J.A., Kaliński, A., Wawrzyniak, J., Bańbura, J., 2009. Opportunity makes a predator: Great Spotted Woodpecker predation on tit broods depends on nest box design. Ornis Fenn. 86, 109-112.
    Slagsvold, T., 1975. Competition between the Great Tit Parus major and the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca in the breeding season. Ornis Scand. 6, 179-190.
    Slevin, M.C., Matthews, A.E., Boves, T.J., 2018. Prothonotary Warbler demography and nest site selection in natural and artificial cavities in bottomland forests of Arkansas, USA. Avian Conserv. Ecol. 13, 5.
    Sloan, S.S., Holmes, R.T., Sherry, T.W., 1998. Depredation rates and predators at artificial bird nests in an unfragmented northern hardwoods forest. J. Wildl. Manag. 62, 529-539.
    Solonen, T., 2001. Breeding of the Great Tit and Blue Tit in urban and rural habitats in southern Finland. Ornis Fenn. 78, 49-60.
    Soltész, Z., Seres, N., Kovács-Hostyánszki, A., 2018. Dipteran assemblages in Red-footed Falcon (Falco vespertinus) nest boxes. Acta Zool. Acad. Sci. Hungar. 64, 91-102.
    Sonerud, G.A., 1989. Reduced predation by Pine Martens on nests of Tengmalm's Owl in relocated boxes. Anim. Behav. 37, 332-334.
    Song, J., 1994. Construction and use of artificial nest boxes. Bull. Biol. 29(4), 25-27+49 (in Chinese).
    Sorace, A., Petrassi, F., Consiglio, C., 2010. Long-distance relocation of nestboxes reduces nest predation by Pine Marten Martes martes. Bird Study 51, 119-124.
    Stamps, J.A., Krishnan, V., 1990. The effect of settlement tactics on territory sizes. Am. Nat. 135, 527-546.
    Stanback, M.T., Mercadante, A.N., Cline, E.L., Burke, T.H., Roth, J.E., 2013. Cavity depth, not experience, determines nest height in Eastern Bluebirds. Wilson J. Ornithol. 125, 301-306.
    Steiger, S.S., Fidler, A.E., Valcu, M., Kempenaers, B., 2008. Avian olfactory receptor gene repertoires: evidence for a well-developed sense of smell in birds? Proc. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 275, 2309-2317.
    Stojanovic, D., Owens, G., Young, C.M., Alves, F., Heinsohn, R., 2020. Do nest boxes breed the target species or its competitors? A case study of a critically endangered bird. Restor. Ecol. 29, e13319.
    Strain, C., Jones, C.S., Griffiths, S.R., Clarke, R.H., 2021. Spout hollow nest boxes provide a drier and less stable microclimate than natural hollows. Conserv. Sci. Pract. 3, e416.
    Swaddle, J.P., Page, L.C., 2007. High levels of environmental noise erode pair preferences in zebra finches: implications for noise pollution. Anim. Behav. 74, 363-368.
    Swinton, S.M., Lupi, F., Robertson, G.P., Hamilton, S.K., 2007. Ecosystem services and agriculture: cultivating agricultural ecosystems for diverse benefits. Ecol. Econ. 64, 245-252.
    Taylor, J., 1976. The advantage of spacing-out. J. Theor. Biol. 59, 485-490.
    Teglhøj, P.G., 2018. Artificial nests for Barn Swallows Hirundo rustica: a conservation option for a declining passerine? Hous. Theor. Soc. 65, 385-395.
    Tomás, G., Merino, S., Martínez De La Puente, J., Moreno, J., Morales, J., Lobato, E., 2008. A simple trapping method to estimate abundances of blood-sucking flying insects in avian nests. Anim. Behav. 75, 723-729.
    Tompa, F.S., 1967. Reproductive success in relation to breeding density in Pied Flycatchers, Ficedula hypoleuca. Acta Zool. Fennica 118, 1-28.
    Tryjanowski, P., Flux, J.E., Sparks, T.H., 2006. Date of breeding of the starling Sturnus vulgaris in New Zealand is related to El Nino Southern Oscillation. Austral Ecol. 31, 634-637.
    Tscharntke, T., Clough, Y., Wanger, T.C., Jackson, L., Motzke, I., Perfecto, I., et al., 2012. Global food security, biodiversity conservation and the future of agricultural intensification. Biol. Conserv. 151, 53-59.
    Verhulst, S., 1998. Multiple breeding in the Great Tit, Ⅱ. The costs of rearing a second clutch. Funct. Ecol. 12, 132-140.
    von Brömssen, A., Jansson, C., 1980. Effects of food addition to Willow Tit Parus montanus and Crested Tit P. cristatus at the time of breeding. Ornis Scand. 11, 173-178.
    Wan, D.M., Liu, Y.N., Zhang, L., Lin, J.F., 2017. The study of cavity-nesting tits selection of nest-box with different entrance size. J. Liaoning Univ. (Nat. Sci. Ed.) 44, 45-50.
    Wang, X.Y., 2011. Breeding ecology of Mandarin Ducks (Aix galericulata). Master’s Thesis. Northeast Normal University, Changchun.
    Wang, H.T., Gao, W., 2002. Utilization of natural cavities by secondary cavity-nesting birds in secondary forest. Zool. Res. 2, 136-140.
    Wang, Y.M., Jiang, S.M., Bao, S.R.L., Zhao, W.J., Wen, G., Lou, Y., et al., 2014. Squirrel breeding habits and growth and development of young in nest boxes. Chin. J. Wildl. 35, 403-406.
    Wang, J.S., Tuanmu, M.N., Hung, C.M., 2021. Effects of artificial light at night on the nest-site selection, reproductive success and behavior of a synanthropic bird. Environ. Pollut. 288, 117805.
    Ware, H.E., Mcclure, C., Carlisle, J.D., Barber, J.R., 2015. A phantom road experiment reveals traffic noise is an invisible source of habitat degradation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112, 12105-12109.
    Webb, D.R., 1987. Thermal tolerance of avian embryos: a review. Condor 89, 874-898.
    Wei, Y.Q., 2014. Artificial nests design method. Master's Thesis. Beijing Forestry University, Beijing.
    Wesołowski, T., 2010. Anti-predator adaptations in nesting Marsh Tits Parus palustris: the role of nest-site security. Ibis 144, 593-601.
    Wesołowski, T., 2011. Reports from nestbox studies: a review of inadequacies. Acta Ornithol. 46, 13-17.
    Wesolowski, T., Czeszczewik, D., Rowiński, P., Walankiewicz, W., 2002. Nest soaking in natural holes - a serious cause of breeding failure? Ornis Fenn. 79, 132-138.
    Wiebe, K.L., 2001. Microclimate of tree cavity nests: is it important for reproductive success in northern flickers? Auk 118, 412-421.
    Wiebe, K.L., 2007. Hypoxia probably does not explain short incubation periods of Woodpeckers. Condor 109, 976-979.
    Wilcove, D.S., 1985. Nest predation in forest tracts and the decline of migratory songbirds. Ecology 66, 1211-1214.
    Willebrand, T., Marcström, V., 1988. On the danger of using dummy nests to study predation. Auk 105, 378-379.
    Wu, K.F., 2011. Diversity survey and artificial attraction evaluation of insectivorous birds in the south part of Xinjiang. Master’s Thesis. Xinjiang Agricultural University, Urumqi.
    Xie, S., Wang, X., Yang, T., Huang, B., Wang, W., Lu, F., et al., 2021. Effect of highways on breeding birds: example of Hulunbeier, China. Glob. Ecol. Conserv. 27, e01554.
    Yom-Tov, Y., Ar, A., 1993. Incubation and fledging durations of woodpeckers. Condor 95, 282-287.
    Yorzinski, J.L., Chisholm, S., Byerley, S.D., Coy, J.R., Aziz, A., Wolf, J.A., et al., 2015. Artificial light pollution increases nocturnal vigilance in peahens. PeerJ 3, e1174.
    You, Y.Y., 2007. The relationship between the reproductive success and habitat quality of the great tit in artificial nests. Master’s Thesis. Northeast Normal University, Changchun.
    Zabotni, A., Kaliński, A., Bańbura, M., Gldalski, M., Bańbura, J., 2020. Experimental nest replacement suggests that the bacterial load of nests may mediate nestling physiological condition in cavity nesting Great Tits (Parus major). J. Ornithol. 161, 819-828.
    Zanette, L., 2002. What do artificial nests tells us about nest predation? Biol. Conserv. 103, 323-329.
    Zhai, L.H., 2007. Study on the Breeding Ecology of Ficedula zanthopypia in Artificial NestBoxes. Master’s Thesis. Northeast Normal University, Changchun.
    Zhang, W., Jia, N.E., Zhao, Y., 2008a. Study on the growth of great tit inside artificial nest-box. J. Yili. Norm. Univ. (Nat. Sci. Ed.). 4, 28-32.
    Zhang, W., Wang, H.T., Yang, Z.J., 2008b. Reproductive parameters of Ficedula zanthopygia in nest-box. Chin. J. Zool. 43, 123-126.
    Zhang, W., Liu, Y., Zuo, B., Han, L., Wang, H., Yang, Z., 2009. Relationship between clutch size and reproductive success of Great Tit breeding inside artificial nest-box. J. N. For. Univ. (Chin. Ed.). 37, 69-71.
    Zhang, K.Q., Gao, W., Deng, Q.X., Liu, J., 2012. Nest-box color preference and reproductive success of Great Tit. Acta Ecol. Sin. 32,659-662.
    Zhang, L., Li, D.L., Ma, R.Q., Xi, C.M., Wan, D.M., 2014. The main nest predators of birds breeding in artificial nest-boxes and its influencing factors. Acta Ecol. Sin. 34, 1235-1243.
    Zhang, D.D., Wang, Y.Y., He, J.J., Wang, S.Q., Zhang, Y.J., Zhao, W.J., 2017. Experimental study of thermal conductivity of artificial nest boxes’ materials in different forms. Shanxi Archit. 43, 200-202.
    Zhao, H., Zhang, K.Q., Wang, H.T., Deng, Q.X., Jiang, Y.L., Liu, J., et al., 2011. Influence of nest box supplement on nature hole selection by secondary cavity-nest birds. Chin. J. Zool. 46, 25-31.
    Zhou, D.Q., Zhou, C.F., Deng, W.H., 2009. Influence of potential cavity resources on secondary cavity-nesters and breeding bird community composition. Biodivers. Sci. 17, 448-457.
    Zhu, J., Guo, J.C., Zhu, M., Wang, Z.M., Wang, B.M., 1998. Study on ecological breeding habits and artificial recruitment techniques of Chinese Nuthatch. Shanxi For. Sci. Tech. 1, 42-47+41.
    Zhu, J.G., Xi, B., Du, Z.Y., Yang, C.B., Zhang, J.F., Huang, T., 2016. Study on bird attraction using artificial nest boxes in Dongzhai national nature reserve. J. Anhui Agr. Sci. 44, 160-161+179.
    Zuo, B., 2005. A preliminary study on the structure of breeding birds community under the influence of artificial nest boxes in the secondary forest. Master’s Thesis. Northeast Normal University, Changchun.
  • Related Articles

Catalog

    Tables(1)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (70) PDF downloads (40) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return