Patrick M. Jagielski, Jennifer R. Foote. 2023: Local and range-wide distribution of song types suggest Ovenbirds (Seiurus aurocapilla) have song neighborhoods but not macro-dialects. Avian Research, 14(1): 100096. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2023.100096
Citation: Patrick M. Jagielski, Jennifer R. Foote. 2023: Local and range-wide distribution of song types suggest Ovenbirds (Seiurus aurocapilla) have song neighborhoods but not macro-dialects. Avian Research, 14(1): 100096. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2023.100096

Local and range-wide distribution of song types suggest Ovenbirds (Seiurus aurocapilla) have song neighborhoods but not macro-dialects

Funds: This research was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC), Canadian Foundation for Innovation (CFI), Ontario Ministry of Research and Innovation (OMRI), and Northern Ontario Heritage Fund Corporation (NOHFC)
More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    E-mail address: jennifer.foote@algomau.ca (J.R. Foote)

  • Received Date: 16 Nov 2022
  • Rev Recd Date: 14 Mar 2023
  • Accepted Date: 14 Mar 2023
  • Available Online: 14 Jun 2023
  • Publish Date: 23 Mar 2023
  • The song systems of oscine passerines (songbirds) are complex and diverse. Because songs are used for both mate attraction and territory defense and are therefore important signals for survival and reproduction, comprehensive knowledge of within and among species song structure and distribution is informative for understanding the evolution of song repertoires and vocal behaviour. In this study, we explored variation in the song structure of the Ovenbird (Seiurus aurocapilla), a widespread warbler (Family Parulidae) found in North American forests. We analyzed recordings from the 2021 breeding season to assess song type variation at a local (n ​= ​158 birds; Sault Ste. Marie, ON) and breeding range scale (n ​= ​512 birds; eBird). We characterized the local song types and tested whether Ovenbirds share song types with their neighbors more often than expected by chance. We then characterized song types of Ovenbirds across the breeding range to determine whether any geographic pattern of song clustering exists (i.e., macro-dialects). We found 10 distinct song types and some evidence for song type clustering at the local study site (i.e., song neighborhoods). We found 7 of those 10 song types throughout the breeding range and identified an additional 24 types that were not recorded in our local population. We found no evidence for song dialects across the Ovenbird breeding range. This study contributes to our understanding of Ovenbird song while simultaneously adding to our understanding of geographic structuring of warbler repertoires. Our work contributes to delineating a more comprehensive understanding of factors affecting dialect development for this diverse group of songbirds.

  • For some waterfowl species, they use diving as the main foraging technique. Diving behavior of waterfowl may be affected by a number of different factors such as prey availability, its distribution, density and type (Benvenuti et al. 2002; Kato et al. 2003; Thompson and Price 2006), water temperature, quality and depth (Guillemette et al. 2004; Green et al. 2005; Thompson and Price 2006), as well as light levels (Wilson et al. 1993; Wanless et al. 1999), body size, physiological functions (Butler and Jones 1982; Sato et al. 2003; Halsey et al. 2006) and habitat type (Ropert-Coudert and Kato 2009). A number of studies have reported that across species, body mass is an important predictor of diving behavior (Trillmich et al. 1991; Costa 1993; Boyd and Croxall 1996; Schreer and Kovacs 1997; Watanuki and Burger 1999). Halsey et al. (2006) analyzed the diving ability of 195 species of birds and mammals and found that both dive and pause durations increased with body mass. Diving theory predicts that large-size birds with a potentially enormous capacity for oxygen storage and low metabolic rate, should be able to stay submerged and surface longer than smaller birds (Cooper 1986). Within a species, however, dive and pause duration of males and adults are usually longer than that of females and juveniles because of different body size (Duncan and Marquiss 1993; Kato et al. 1999; Polak and Ciach 2007; Gomes et al. 2009; Laich et al. 2012).

    The Scaly-sided Merganser (Mergus squamatus) is listed in the first category of nationally protected wildlife species in China and is regarded as an endangered species by the IUCN in 2002 (Wang and Xie 2009). This species mainly breeds in southeastern Russia, North Korea and Heilongjiang, Jilin and Inner Mongolia in the northeast of China (Birdlife International 2014). Wintering areas include a wide stretch along the south bank of the Yangtze River (He et al. 2006), while the north of Jiangxi Province provides one of the major wintering habitats for this species in China. Its population is found mainly along four rivers of Jiangxi, i.e., the Xinjiang, Fuhe, Raohe and Xiuhe rivers and their tributaries (Liu et al. 2008; Zeng et al. 2013; Shao et al. 2014). At a global level, their number is about 1200 pairs of which about 250 individual birds are wintering in Jiangxi. At present, the population of the Scaly-sided Merganser continues to decline due to a very high level of habitat loss, illegal hunting and human interference (Birdlife International 2014 ). Although a number of previous studies about wintering ecology of the Scaly-sided Merganser have been carried out (Liu et al. 2008; Shao et al. 2010; Yi et al. 2010), relatively little is known about their diving behavior and the factors that impact this species during winter (Shao et al. 2014).

    From December 2015 to March 2016, we studied the diving behavior of the wintering Scaly-sided Merganser in Wuyuan, Jiangxi. The objectives of this study were to (1) provide basic data on diving behavior of wintering Scaly-sided Merganser, and (2) reveal the effect of sex, temperature, time and flock size on its diving behavior.

    Wuyuan County is located in the northeast of Jiangxi Province, China. This area has a humid subtropical climate with an average annual temperature of 16.7 ℃ (Shao et al. 2012) and the average annual precipitation is 1816 mm (Ji et al. 2014). Our study site was the Raohe River in Wuyuan County (29°9′24″-29°11′15″N, 117°50′01″‒117°51′06″E) (Zeng et al. 2013) (Fig. 1). The vegetation is dominated by broad-leaved deciduous forests and mixed broad-leaved and evergreen mountain forests. Scattered farmland and villages are distributed in this area. The main activity range of our merganser is focused on about 7 km of river banks. They are also found dispersed at other river sections or in company of other birds along other stretches of the river. The population of the Scaly-sided Merganser we studied consisted of about 20 individuals during one wintering period. Sympatric waterfowl species in this area include Chinese Spot-billed Duck (Anas poecilorhyncha), Mallard (Anas platyrhynchos), Mandarin Duck (Aix galericulata), Little Grebe (Tachybaptus ruficollis), Little Egret (Egretta garzetta), Green Sandpiper (Tringa ochropus), Common Sandpiper (Actitis hypoleucos) and Eurasian Coot (Fulica atra).

    Figure  1.  Study area of the Scaly-sided Merganser (Mergus squamatus)

    We used a focal sampling method to enumerate the diving activities of the Scaly-sided Merganser in the Wuyuan section of the Poyang Lake watershed, from December 2015 to March 2016. Data were collected with binoculars (30×) and a spotting scope (20‒60×) from 7:30 hours to 17:30 hours. In total 34 days of field investigations were carried out in our study area, involving 8 days in December, 7 in January, 9 in February and 10 days in March. In order to estimate the effect of various factors on the diving behavior of the merganser, we collected and compared diving data by sex, temperature (daily average air temperature), time (time of year and daytime) and flock size. We determined the sex by the feather color of its head or back. Data recorded include sex, temperature (once every half hour), flock size, start time and end time of diving, dive duration and pause time between consecutive dives. Diving efficiency, defined as the dive/pause (d/p) ratio, was calculated from the dive duration and the subsequent pause duration (Dewar 1924). We also tried to collect data at different time and from various observation points to decrease the possibility that we monitored the same individual bird more than once. In this paper, daytime means the different time period from 7:30 hours to 17:30 hours in diurnal scanning time.

    One-way ANOVAs and LSD multiple comparisons were used to test the differences between each group (Jiang and Shao 2015). The Pearson correlation test was used to test the correlation between pause duration and the previous and subsequent dive duration. The Spearman correlation test was used to test correlations between temperature, time of year, daytime, flock size, dive and pause duration. All our calculated results are expressed as mean ± standard deviation (x¯±SD), with p values < 0.05 considered significant. All statistical analyses were completed in Excel 2010 and SPSS 22.0.

    A total of 201 focals and 37.14 h were performed and all focals were performed by the same person. During feeding, the Scaly-sided Merganser remained submerged for 2.5-34.7 s and paused on the surface between dives for 1.2-94.5 s, with diving efficiency ranging from 0.068 to 13.429. Mean dive duration was 18.8 ± 6.0 s (n = 4305) versus a mean pause duration of 12.9 ± 11.2 s (n = 4104), with a diving efficiency of 2.297 ± 1.626 (n = 4104) (Table 1). Pause duration was positively correlated with the previous dive duration (r = 0.099, p < 0.001, n = 4104), as well as with the duration of the subsequent dive (r = 0.094, p < 0.001, n = 4104).

    Table  1.  Diving parameters of wintering Scaly-sided Mergansers between sexes
    Sex Number of focals Number of dives Dive duration (s) Number of pauses Pause duration (s) Diving efficiency
    Male103211720.1 ± 6.2a 201413.6 ± 12.0a 2.401 ± 1.732a
    Female98218817.5 ± 5.4b 209012.2 ± 10.4b 2.197 ± 1.510b
    Total201430518.8 ± 6.0410412.9 ± 11.22.297 ± 1.626
    Values with the same letter indicate no significant difference (p > 0.05), while different letters indicate significant differences (p < 0.05), also applying to the tables below
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Mean dive duration (F = 223.373, df = 1, p < 0.001), mean time on the surface (F = 16.460, df = 1, p < 0.001) and diving efficiency (F = 16.189, df = 1, p < 0.001) of males were significantly longer or higher than the estimates for females (Table 1).

    Significant differences were detected among the mean dive duration (F = 239.418, df = 2, p < 0.001) and mean time on the surface (F = 3.331, df = 2, p < 0.05) within the range of observed temperatures (Table 2). The mean dive duration was significantly longer when the temperature was 4-10 ℃ while was significantly shorter at 11-19 ℃. With the rise of temperature, the mean dive duration showed a significant trend of an initial decrease and a subsequent increase in submergence. Mean pause duration was highest when the temperature was 22-29 ℃ in contrast to the 4-10 ℃ range. With the rise of temperature, the mean pause duration showed no significant change. Diving efficiency was significantly higher when the temperature was 4-10 ℃ than at other temperatures, while it was lowest between 22 and 29 ℃. Dive duration was negatively correlated with the temperature (r = -0.269, p < 0.001) (Table 2).

    Table  2.  Diving parameters of wintering Scaly-sided Mergansers at different air temperatures
    Temperature (℃) Number of divesDive duration (s) Number of pausesPause duration (s) Diving efficiency
    4‒10153021.3 ± 5.5a 143012.4 ± 10.1a 2.579 ± 1.647a
    11‒19165317.2 ± 5.5b 158812.9 ± 11.4a 2.176 ± 1.670b
    22‒29112217.7 ± 6.1c 108613.5 ± 12.3a 2.105 ± 1.478b
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Significant differences were detected among the mean dive duration (F = 195.420, df = 3, p < 0.001) and the mean time on the surface (F = 23.652, df = 3, p < 0.001) over various months (Table 3). The mean dive duration in December and January was significantly higher than in February and March. The mean pause duration in January was significantly lower than in other months. Diving efficiency in January was significantly higher than in other months, contrast to that in February. The time of year from December 2015 to March 2016 was negatively correlated with dive duration (r = -0.176, p < 0.001) (Table 3).

    Table  3.  Diving parameters of wintering Scaly-sided Mergansers at different wintering periods
    Period/daytime Number of dives Dive duration (s) Number of pauses Pause duration (s) Diving efficiency
    Month
    Dec. 84221.1 ± 5.1a 77513.1 ± 8.2a 2.136 ± 1.149a
    Jan. 74321.0 ± 6.3a 7059.7 ± 10.5b 3.360 ± 1.985b
    Feb. 152116.2 ± 5.3b 146713.7 ± 11.2a 1.903 ± 1.522c
    Mar. 119919.1 ± 5.8c 115713.7 ± 13.0a 2.503 ± 1.983d
    Daytime
    7:30‒8:5942119.4 ± 5.3a 39716.4 ± 13.1a 1.976 ± 1.555a
    9:00‒15:59375518.8 ± 6.0a 358212.6 ± 11.0b 2.350 ± 1.646b
    16:00‒17:3012914.9 ± 5.8b 12511.2 ± 9.8b 1.811 ± 0.933a
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Significant differences were detected among mean dive duration (F = 29.517, df = 2, p < 0.001), mean time on the surface (F = 21.922, df = 2, p < 0.001) and diving efficiency (F = 21.922, df = 2, p < 0.001) at various daytime (Table 3). The mean dive duration and mean pause time between 7:30 and 8:59 was longest, while shortest between 16:00 and 17:30. Diving efficiency was highest between 9:00 and 15:59 hours, contrast to that between 16:00 and 17:30 hours. Daytime was negatively correlated with dive duration (r = -0.081, p < 0.001) and pause duration (r = -0.101, p < 0.001) while it was positively correlated with diving efficiency (r = 0.052, p = 0.001) (Table 3).

    Significant differences were observed among mean dive duration (F = 106.694, df = 3, p < 0.001) and mean time on the surface (F = 5.106, df = 3, p < 0.05) for various flock sizes (Table 4). With flock sizes between 1 and 5 birds, the mean dive duration was significantly higher than that of any other flock size, while between 16 and 19 birds, the mean dive duration was significantly less than that of any other flock size. With an increase in the flock size, the mean dive duration showed a significantly decreasing trend. With flock sizes between 16 and 19 birds, the mean pause duration was significantly less than that of any other flock size. With an increase in flock size, the mean pause duration showed a gradually decreasing trend. Diving efficiency was significantly higher in flock sizes between 1 and 5 birds than in any other flock size, while between 11 and 15 birds, diving efficiency was significantly less than in any other flock size. Flock size was negatively correlated with dive duration (r = -0.259, p < 0.001) (Table 4).

    Table  4.  Diving parameters of wintering Scaly-sided Mergansers among different flock sizes
    Flock size Number of dives Dive duration (s) Number of pauses Pause duration (s) Diving efficiency
    1‒5235419.9 ± 5.9a 224913.2 ± 12.5a 2.546 ± 1.786a
    6‒1067519.0 ± 5.8b 63813.0 ± 9.8a 2.127 ± 1.399b
    11‒1591217.0 ± 5.4c 86913.0 ± 9.2a 1.849 ± 1.240c
    16‒1936415.3 ± 5.7d 34810.7 ± 9.0b 2.130 ± 1.478b
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    In our study, we found that mean dive duration of the Scaly-sided Merganser was shorter than that studied in Yihuang County (23.6 s) (Yihuang County is located in the Fuhe River valley), while mean pause duration was similar to that study (11.6 s) (Shao et al. 2014). The relationship between this difference and water depth, access to food sources, type and availability in the two water areas we investigated requires further study (Benvenuti et al. 2002; Kato et al. 2003; Green et al. 2005; Thompson and Price 2006). Dive durations of the Scaly-sided Merganser were similar to the short time bursts, i.e., 17.5-24.3 s, reported for Barrow's Goldeneyes (Bucephala islandica) (Bourget et al. 2007), Common Goldeneyes (Bucephala clangula) (19.7‒23.9 s) (Bourget et al. 2007) and the Great Crested Grebe (Podiceps cristatus) with 19.5 s (Gomes et al. 2009), as well as that for small diving birds, such as the Little Grebe (Tachybaptus ruficollis) with 13.84 s (Jiang and Shao 2015). These short time bursts were considerably shorter than those reported for large diving birds such as the Subantarctic Cormorant (Phalacrocorax albiventer) with 67‒108 s (Kato et al. 1999), Rock shags (Phalacrocorax magellanicus) with 47.2 s (Quintana 1999) and Red-legged Cormorants (Phalacrocorax gaimardi) with 26.8 s (Frere et al. 2002). Pause durations of the Scaly-sided Merganser were similar to those reported for Barrow's Goldeneyes (12.4‒14.0 s), Common Goldeneyes (10.4‒12.8 s) and the Tufted Duck (Aythya fuligula) (12.96 s) (Halsey et al. 2005). These differences may be related to their body mass, an opinion supported by Cooper's prediction (Cooper 1986).

    Diving efficiency (2.297) of our Scaly-sided Merganser was similar to Yihuang section in Fuhe River (1.88‒2.21) while higher than that of the Great Crested Grebe (1.2) (Gomes et al. 2009) and the Little Grebe (1.13) (Jiang and Shao 2015). The American coot (Fulica Americana) enhances its diving efficiency when access to food sources decreases due to prolonged dive duration and shortened pause duration. These differences in diving efficiency may be caused by water depth, light levels and the ecological habitat of the species (Jiang and Shao 2015). Pause duration was positively correlated to earlier dive durations as well as to the duration of the subsequent dive; this agreed with the results obtained by Shao et al. (2014) and those of Jiang and Shao (2015), whose results suggest that the time spent on the surface is a period which the bird uses to recover from the previous dive and to prepare for the next one.

    Mean dive duration and mean time on the surface of male Scaly-sided mergansers were significantly longer than those for females and similar to those of Common Goldeneyes (Duncan and Marquiss 1993), the Subantarctic Cormorant, the Japanese Cormorant (Kato et al. 1999) and the Blue-eyed Shag (Laich et al. 2012). These results may be related to body mass differences (Scaly-sided Merganser: W: 1.0‒1.2 kg, W: 0.8‒1.0 kg; Common Goldeneyes: W: 0.8‒1.0 kg, W: 0.5‒0.9 kg; Subantarctic Cormorant: W: 2.9 kg, W: 2.5 kg; Japanese Cormorant: W: 3.1 kg, W: 2.5 kg; Blue-eyed Shag: W significantly larger than W) (Kato et al. 1999; Zhao 2001; Laich et al. 2012) and accord with Cooper's prediction (Cooper 1986).

    Dive duration was negatively correlated to the average daily air temperature, which was similar to the results obtained by Green et al. (2005) and may be related to the minimum rate of oxygen consumption. This minimum rate was far smaller in relatively low temperatures than in high temperatures; therefore, with a decrease in the energy cost of diving, the dive duration increased (Green et al. 2005).

    Dive duration was negatively related to time of year and daytime. The temperature in December and January were lower than in February and March in our study area. Dive duration was significantly higher in December and January, which may be related to the low minimum rate of oxygen consumption for diving waterfowl under low temperature conditions (Green et al. 2005). However, the temperature in February was lower than in March while dive duration was significantly lower than in March. This unique result, appearing in February, may be caused by increased human activity during China's traditional spring festival. Studies have shown that the diving behavior of waterfowl is limited by light conditions, with dive durations longer at lower light intensity (Wilson et al. 1993; Wanless et al. 1999). Our study supports this general assumption. The longest mean dive duration of the Scaly-sided Merganser was from 7:30‒8:59 when light intensity was low. The dive duration of Scaly-sided Mergansers at 16:00‒17:30 was significantly lower than at any other time, which may be related to their ecological habits or prey behavior.

    During this study, we found that the foraging time of our mergansers clearly increased and their vigilance decreased when the flock size was larger, which agrees with the point of view by Roberts (1996) on this issue. That is, with a larger flock, the probability of detecting a predator for individual bird increases while the risk of becoming prey diminishes. That leads to an increase in the foraging time for individual birds and a decrease in the necessity for vigilance (Clark and Mangel 1986). Dive duration of the Scaly-sided Merganser was negatively correlated to flock size, i.e., with a larger flock the dive duration shortens significantly, thereby increasing diving time considerably, quite in agreement with the hypothesis of competitive disturbance. In an environment of limited food sources and larger flocks, individual birds will increase their speed in foraging in order to acquire more food, leading to an increase in forage activity for individual birds and a decrease in vigilance (Clark and Mangel 1986). Furthermore, intraspecific competition was more intense when the group of Scaly-sided Mergansers was larger. In order to acquire more food effectively and decrease intraspecific competition, the Scaly-sided Merganser has adopted a flexible foraging strategy that shortens dive duration and adds to diving times. With an enlargement of flock size, the predation ability and efficiency increases due to collective predation, allowing the Scaly-sided Merganser to acquire more prey in a limited time.

    Sex, temperature, time and flock size are factors that affect the diving behavior of our wintering Scaly-sided Merganser. The mean dive duration and mean time on the surface of males were significantly higher than those of females, which may be related to the weight differences between sexes. With an increase in temperatures, dive duration increased considerably, along with the passage of time of year and daytime. The difference of diving behavior at various temperatures and daytime may be related to a low minimum rate of oxygen consumption. With an increase in flock size, dive duration decreased significantly and may be caused by rising interspecific competition.

    MQ and BC conceived and designed the experiments. BC performed the experiments and BC and MQ analyzed the data. BC and MQ wrote the paper. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

    The work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 31560597).

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

  • Baker, M.C., Gammon, D.E., 2008. Vocal memes in natural populations of chickadees: why do some memes persist and others go extinct? Anim. Behav. 75, 279-289.
    Baptista, L.F., Morton, M.L., 1988. Song learning in montane white-crowned sparrows: from whom and when. Anim. Behav. 36, 1753-1764.
    Beecher, M.D., Brenowitz, E.A., 2005. Functional aspects of song learning in songbirds. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 143-149.
    Bell, D., Slabbekoorn, H., Jesse, A., 2003. Microgeographic song variation in island populations of the white-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys nutalli): innovation through recombination. Behaviour 140, 947-963.
    Bryan, K., Moldenhauer, R., Kroodsma, D.E., 1987. Geographic uniformity in songs of the Prothonotary Warbler. Wilson Bull. 99, 369-376.
    Canty, A., Ripley, B., 2021. Boot: Bootstrap R (S-Plus) Functions. R Package Version 1, pp. 3–28.
    Catchpole, C.K., Slater, P.J., 2003. Bird song: biological themes and variations. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
    Ehnes, M., Foote, J.R., 2015. Comparison of autonomous and manual recording methods for discrimination of individually distinctive Ovenbird songs. Bioacoustics 24, 111-121.
    Hann, H.W., 1937. Life history of the Oven-bird in southern Michigan. Wilson Bull. 49, 145-237.
    Hannah, K.C., Bayne, E.M., Sánchez, N.V., 2020. First description of the structure and geographic patterns in the songs of the Connecticut Warbler (Oporornis agilis). Wilson J. Ornithol. 132, 598-607.
    Heinemann, D., 1981. Song dialects, migration, and population structure of Puget Sound white-crowned sparrows. Auk 98, 512-521.
    Hensel, A.L., Dobney, S.L., Doucet, S.M., Norris, D.R., Newman, A.E., Williams, H., et al., 2022. Microgeographical variation in birdsong: Savannah sparrows exhibit microdialects in an island population. Anim. Behav. 188, 119-131.
    Holmes, S.B., Sanders, C.J., Fillman, D., Welsh, D.A., 2009. Changes in a forest bird community during an outbreak cycle of the spruce budworm in northwestern Ontario. Bird Popul. 9, 13-28.
    Ince, S.A., Slater, P.J.B., Weismann, C., 1980. Changes with time in the songs of a population of chaffinches. Condor 82, 285-290.
    Janes, S.W., Ryker, L., 2016. Nashville Warblers (Oreothlypis ruficapilla) use a single song type in southwestern Oregon with widely distributed song variants. Wilson J. Ornithol. 128, 412-418.
    Ju, C., Geller, F.C., Mundinger, P.C., Lhati, D.C., 2019. Four decades of cultural evolution in House Finch songs. Auk 136, uky012.
    Kroodsma, D.E., 2004. The diversity and plasticity of birdsong. In: Marler, P., Slabbekoorn, H. (Eds.), Nature's Music: the Science of Birdsong. Elsevier, Cambridge, MA, pp. 108–131.
    Kroodsma, D.E., 1980. Winter Wren singing behavior: a pinnacle of song complexity. Condor 82, 357-365.
    Lambrechts, M., Dhondt, A.A., 1986. Male quality, reproduction, and survival in the great tit (Parus major). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 19, 57-63.
    Lein, M.R., 1981. Display behavior of Ovenbirds (Seiurus aurocapillus) Ⅱ. Song variation and singing behavior. Wilson Bull. 93, 21-41.
    Lemon, R.E., 1975. How birds develop song dialects. Condor 77, 385-406.
    Liu, W.C., Kroodsma, D.E., 2006. Song learning by chipping sparrows: when, where, and from whom. Condor 108, 509-517.
    Marler, P., 1970. A comparative approach to vocal learning: song development in white-crowned sparrows. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 71, 1-25.
    Marler, P., Tamura, M., 1962. Song “dialects” in three populations of White-crowned Sparrows. Condor 4, 368-377.
    McGregor, P.K., 1991. The singer and the song: on the receiving end of bird song. Biol. Rev. 66, 57-81.
    McGregor, P.K., Krebs, J.R., 1982. Song types in a population of great tits (Parus major): their distribution, abundance and acquisition by individuals. Behaviour 79, 126-152.
    Mehta C.R., Patel, N.R., 1983. A network algorithm for performing Fisher's exact test in r × c contingency tables. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 78, 427-434.
    Naguib, M., Riebel, K., 2014. Singing in space and time: the biology of birdsong. In: Witzany, G. (Ed.), Biocommunication of Animals. Springer, Dordrecht, pp. 233–247.
    Nelson, D.A., Hallberg, K.I., Soha, J.A., 2004. Cultural evolution of Puget sound white-crowned sparrow song dialects. Ethology 110, 879-908.
    Nelson, D.A., Marler, P., 1994. Selection-based learning in bird song development. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91, 10498-10501.
    Nowicki, S., Searcy, W.A., Hughes, M., Podos, J., 2001. The evolution of bird song: male and female response to song innovation in swamp sparrows. Anim. Behav. 62, 1189-1195.
    Otter, K.A., Mckenna, A., LaZerte, S.E., Ramsay, S.M., 2020. Continent-wide shifts in song dialects of white-throated sparrows. Curr. Biol. 30, 3231-3235.
    Payne, R.B., Payne, L.L., Doehlert, S.M., 1988. Biological and cultural success of song memes in indigo buntings. Ecology 69, 104-117.
    Peters, S., Nowicki, S., 2017. Overproduction and attrition: the fates of songs memorized during song learning in songbirds. Anim. Behav. 124, 255-261.
    Pitocchelli, J., Guerra, D., Kender, J., 2018. Macrogeographic variation in song of the MacGillivray’s Warbler (Geothlypis tolmiei). Wilson J. Ornithol. 130, 716-729.
    Porneluzi, P., Van Horn, M.A., Donovan, T.M., 2020. Ovenbird (Seiurus aurocapilla), version 1.0. In: Poole, A.F. (Ed.), Birds of the World. Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY. .
    R Core Team, 2021. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL .
    Searcy, W.A., McArthur, P.D., Yasukawa, K., 1985. Song repertoire size and male quality in song sparrows. Condor 87, 222-228.
    Searcy, W.A., Andersson, M., 1986. Sexual selection and the evolution of song. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Systemat. 17, 507-533.
    Searfoss, A.M., Liu, W.C., Creanz, N., 2020. Geographically well-distributed citizen science data reveals range-wide variation in the chipping sparrow’s simple song. Anim. Behav. 161, 63-76.
    Shonfield, J., Bayne, E., 2017. Autonomous recording units in avian ecological research: current use and future applications. Avian Conserv. Ecol. 12, 14.
    Slater, P.J., 1989. Bird song learning: causes and consequences. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 1, 19-46.
    Slater, P.J.B., Ince, S.A., 1979. Cultural evolution in chaffinch song. Behaviour 71, 146-166.
    Smith, S.M., 1991. The Black-Capped Chickadee: Behavioral Ecology and Natural History. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY.
    Spector, D.A., 1992. Wood-warbler song systems. In: Power, D.M. (Ed.), Current Ornithology, 9. Springer, Boston, pp. 199–238.
    Sullivan, B.L., Aycrigg, J.L., Barry, J.H., Bonney, R.E., Bruns, N., Cooper, C.B., et al., 2014. The eBird enterprise: an integrated approach to development and application of citizen science. Biol. Conserv. 169, 31-40.
    Thomas, I.P., Doucet, S.M., Norris, D.R., Newman, A.E., Williams, H., Mennill, D.J., 2021. Vocal learning in Savannah sparrows: acoustic similarity to neighbours shapes song development and territorial aggression. Anim. Behav. 176, 77-86.
    Thompson, M.J., Pearse, K.A., Foote, J.R., 2020. Seasonal and diel plasticity of song type use in individual ovenbirds (Seiurus aurocapilla). Ethology 126, 824-838.
    Tsipoura, N., Morton, E.S., 1988. Song-type distribution in a population of Kentucky Warblers. Wilson Bull. 100, 9-16.
    Venier, L.A., Holmes, S.B., 2010. A review of the interaction between forest birds and eastern spruce budworm. Environ. Rev. 18, 191-207.
    Weeden, J.S., Falls, J.B., 1959. Differential responses of male ovenbirds to recorded songs of neighboring and more distant individuals. Auk 76, 343-351.
    Wheelwright, N.T., Swett, M.B., Levin, I.I., Kroodsma, D.E., Freeman-Gallant, C.R., Williams, H., 2008. The influence of different tutor types on song learning in a natural bird population. Anim. Behav. 75, 1479-1493.
    Wilson, P.L., Towner, M.C., Vehrencamp, S.L., 2000. Survival and song-type sharing in a sedentary subspecies of the song sparrow. Condor 102, 355-363.
    Wildenthal, J.L., 1965. Structure in primary song of the mockingbird (Mimus polyglottos). Auk 82, 161-189.
    Williams, H., 2004. Birdsong and singing behavior. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1016, 1-30.
    Williams, H., Robins, C.W., Norris, D.R., Newman, A.E.M., Freeman-Gallant, C.R., Wheelwright, N.T., et al., 2019. The buzz segment of Savannah sparrow song is a population marker. J. Ornithol. 160, 217-227.
    Williams, H., Schar, A., Ryba, A.R., Norris, D.R., Mennill, D.J., Newman, A.E.M., et al., 2022. Cumulative cultural evolution and mechanisms for cultural selection in wild bird songs. Nat. Commun. 13, 4001.
    Wright, T.F., Dahlin, C.R., Salinas-Melgoza, A., 2008. Stability and change in vocal dialects of the yellow-naped amazon. Anim. Behav. 76, 1017-1027.
  • Related Articles

Catalog

    Figures(6)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (30) PDF downloads (20) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return