Sanja Barišić, Davor Ćiković, Vesna Tutiš, Jelena Kralj, Herbert Hoi. 2022: Context dependent song-flight performance and timing in the Black-headed Bunting (Emberiza melanocephala). Avian Research, 13(1): 100059. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2022.100059
Citation: Sanja Barišić, Davor Ćiković, Vesna Tutiš, Jelena Kralj, Herbert Hoi. 2022: Context dependent song-flight performance and timing in the Black-headed Bunting (Emberiza melanocephala). Avian Research, 13(1): 100059. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2022.100059

Context dependent song-flight performance and timing in the Black-headed Bunting (Emberiza melanocephala)

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    E-mail address: sbarisic@hazu.hr (S. Barišić)

  • Received Date: 17 Mar 2022
  • Rev Recd Date: 04 Aug 2022
  • Accepted Date: 18 Aug 2022
  • Available Online: 11 Jan 2023
  • Publish Date: 05 Sep 2022
  • Many songbirds produce song-flights; however, the function, vocal and motor characteristics, as well as the diel and seasonal variation of song-flight in songbirds remain not well understood. Here, we studied two types of song-flight in male Black-headed Buntings (Emberiza melanocephala), the Moth — a standard, i.e., perch song produced during a horizontal flight, and the Towering — a specifically structured song produced during a complex ascending and descending flight. While perch song, used during Moth flight, has already been described, here we provide the first description and sonogram representation of the more elaborate and less stereotyped Towering song. While males started to perform Moth song-flights as soon as they arrived at the breeding site, Towering did not start before the female arrival. Males usually delivered spontaneous Moth song-flights before chasing rivals or undertaking aerial fights, suggesting Moth is directed towards conspecific males and serves as a threat display. Furthermore, playback of conspecific perch songs triggered males to approach the loudspeaker with Moth song-flights. The Towering started after females arrived at the breeding site, suggesting the Towering is directed towards conspecific females. We detected a seasonal difference in the peak Towering rate between forenoon and afternoon, suggesting that Towerings performed at different times of the day have different functions. Forenoon Towerings were strongly positively related to the number of fertile females at the breeding site, while afternoon Towerings peaked very early in the breeding season and coincided with the number of females at the breeding site before the onset of nest-building. These results provide evidence that birds can direct different types of song-flight towards different receivers and suggest the possibility that time of the day determines the context of the display, namely to attract social mates or solicit copulation. Further work is needed to account for the effect of between male variability in song-flight rate, as well as in height/steepness of flight or song quality.

  • Trait polymorphism in natural populations can evolve as a consequence of frequency-dependent selection (Majerus, 1998). This implies that parasites, predators or other selective agents impose variable intensities of selection on the phenotype depending on the frequency in the population. Brood parasites and their hosts provide one such possible case of frequencydependent selection resulting in the evolution of polymorphic eggs in both host and parasite (Kilner, 2006; Yang et al., 2010a). Obligate avian brood parasites lay their eggs in nests belonging to other species of birds, thereby transferring the costs of parental care to their victims. As a consequence, hosts evolve defenses to counter brood parasitism, which in turn selects for corresponding counter-adaptations for better trickery of parasites (Davies and Brooke, 1989). The well-known arms race between parasitic cuckoos and their hosts are regarded as a textbook example of co-evolutionary interactions. Theoretically, the cuckoo-host system, when acting in a frequency-dependent manner, should be able to produce polymorphisms in co-evolved traits in the interacting parties. This hypothetical scenario has been found in the Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) and one of its hosts, the Ashy-throated Parrotbill (Paradoxornis alphonsianus), in which both species have evolved matching egg polymorphism manifested in discrete immaculate white, pale blue and blue egg phenotypes within a single population (Fig. 1; Yang et al., 2010a). However, egg mimicry assessment is not always straightforward. Inspection using spectrophotometric methods suggested that the eggs of the Great Spotted Cuckoo (Clamator glandarius) were not significantly related to the appearance of its Magpie (Pica pica) host eggs (Soler et al., 2003). In the Red-chested Cuckoo (Cuculus solitarius), cuckoo eggs actually match the eggs of their hosts most closely at wavelengths that cannot be perceived by the human eye (Cherry and Bennett, 2001). Starling et al. (2006) revealed by reflectance spectrophotometry that the color of Pallid Cuckoo (Cuculus pallidus) eggs differed between four host species of Melaphagid Honeyeaters (Lichenostomus penicillatus, L. chrysops, L. melanops, and Melithreptus affinis), and mimicked their hosts' eggs closely in both spectral shape and brightness. The Pallid Cuckoo eggs from the four different hosts' nests matched their respective hosts closely. However, host eggs exhibited a small peak in the ultraviolet that was not mimicked by the cuckoo eggs (Starling et al., 2006). Using digital image analysis and modelling of avian vision, Stoddard and Stevens (2010) recently showed that various features of host egg pattern are mimicked by the eggs of their respective cuckoo host-race. These studies revealed that cuckoos have host-specific egg types that have not been detected by human observation, and emphasize potential inadequacy of human comparisons applied to the coloration of bird eggs, and the importance of techniques such as spectrophotometry to measure color objectively (Starling et al., 2006).

    Figure  1.  Egg polymorphism of Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) and Ashy-throated Parrotbill (Paradoxornis alphonsianus). (a), (b) and (c) refer to blue, pale blue and white clutches of parrotbill, respectively, with a cuckoo egg (larger egg) being present in each clutch (Photos by C. Yang).

    The objective of this study was to quantify egg color by spectrophotometry and assess the extent of egg mimicry of Common Cuckoo to the eggs of its Ashythroated Parrotbill host for blue, pale blue and white clutches, respectively.

    The study was performed in the Kuankuoshui Nature Reserve, Guizhou, south-western China (28°10′N, 107° 10′E) during April–July 2008–2009. The study site is situated in a subtropical moist broadleaf and mixed forest, interspersed with abandoned tea plantations, shrubby areas, and open fields used as cattle pastures (see also Yang et al., 2010a, b).

    Nests were found by systematically searching all typical and potential nest sites and by monitoring the activities of adult hosts throughout the breeding season. We recorded date of the first egg laid, egg color morph, clutch size and occurrence of brood parasitism for each nest. When a nest was found during the incubation period, eggs were floated in water to estimate approximate laying date (Hays and Lecroy, 1971). We used three spectrophotometers for quantification of egg coloration: the USB4000-VIS-NIR, GZ03P and Avantes-2048 to measure the visible (VIS) range (400–700 nm) of blue and white clutches, ultraviolet (UV) range (300–400 nm) of blue and white clutches (Fig. 2) and VIS-UV range (300–700 nm) of pale blue clutches (Fig. 3), respectively (Yang et al., 2009, 2011). Due to equipment limit, we did in such way, which was surely a suboptimal way of doing it. In earlier years, our spectrophotometer can only measure the spectrum range from 400 to 700 nm (VIS). And an additional UV spectrophotometer was used to supplement the UV data. But these data are from quite different machines and cannot be merged together. Finally, the pale blue eggs were measured by the Avantes spectrophotometer which covers the spectrum range from 300–700 nm. However, cuckoo eggs were few and phenotypes we found were very variable in different years.

    Figure  2.  Ultraviolet and visible reflectance spectrum of the egg phenotypes in Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) and Ashy-throated Parrotbill (Paradoxornis alphonsianus). The curves represent the spectrum for one cuckoo egg and average spectra for all host eggs in the observed parasitized nest. B1 and B2 refer to the UV and VIS spectrum of blue clutches; W1 and W2 refer to the UV and VIS spectrum of white clutches.
    Figure  3.  Ultraviolet and visible reflectance spectra of the pale blue egg phenotype in Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) and Ashythroated Parrotbill (Paradoxornis alphonsianus). The curves represent the spectrum for one cuckoo egg and average spectra for all host eggs in the observed parasitized nest.

    Both the Ashy-throated Parrotbill (hereafter parrotbill) and the Common Cuckoo (hereafter cuckoo) laid immaculate eggs (Fig. 1), and we obtained six measurements of spectral reflectance for each egg, with two at the blunt end, two at the middle and two at the sharp end of the egg. To represent the egg coloration of the cuckoo, the mean of each egg was summarized from these six measurements. For the parrotbill, egg coloration was represented as the mean of all host eggs in each clutch. Each measurement covered ca. 1 mm2 and was taken at a 45° angle to the egg surface, with the spectrometer and light source connected with a coaxial reflectance probe (Yang et al., 2009, 2010b). We also classified the degree of cuckoo eggs mimicry on a 5-degree scale based on human vision relying on 30 volunteers who scored the degree of mimicry (contrast) from 1 (non-mimetic) to 5 (perfect mimicry) following the approach first developed by Moksnes and Røskaft (1995).

    The experiments comply with the current laws of China in which they were performed. Experimental procedures were in agreement with the Animal Research Ethics Committee of Hainan Provincial Education Centre for Ecology and Environment, Hainan Normal University.

    Data analyses were performed in SPSS 13.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Illinois). One-way ANOVA and Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA were used for comparison of normally and non-normally distributed data, respectively. Values were presented as mean ± SD.

    Mimicry score based on human vision showed that the contrasts between cuckoo and parrotbill eggs of the matched-phenotype (blue versus blue, pale blue versus pale blue, and white versus white) differed significantly among the three egg phenotypes (χ2 = 4.41, df = 2, p = 0.015). The mimicry of blue cuckoo eggs to blue host eggs was the highest and significantly higher than that of the white matched pair (blue: 1.03 ± 0.18 vs. white: 1.30 ± 0.47, n = 30 for each category, p = 0.015, post hoc test). The mimicry of pale blue cuckoo egg was intermediate between the blue and the white egg (1.13 ± 0.35, n = 30), with no statistical significant difference from blue (p = 0.273) or white eggs (p = 0.070).

    Egg reflectance spectra revealed that the wave shape, wave peak and wave trough of cuckoo and parrotbill egg spectrum for the blue phenotype were perfectly matching in both visible (VIS) and ultraviolet (UV) ranges (Figs. 12), which indicated that they were very similar in egg color hue and chroma. However, the wave shape of the white cuckoo egg was more variable, with a wave peak in the blue region (Fig. 2), which were lacking in the parrotbill egg. The reflectance spectra for UV between white cuckoo and parrotbill eggs were discrete in 300–340 nm. A similar pattern was found for pale blue cuckoo and parrotbill eggs for which the reflectance curves matched well in wavelengths 340–700 nm.

    Our results show that egg reflectance spectra agree well with the assessment based on human vision that cuckoo eggs mimic those of the parrotbill host. Our previous studies have also indicated that the classification of parrotbill egg morphs based on human vision is consistent with avian visual modelling (Yang et al., 2010a). The sensitivities of UVS-receptor of many birds are concentrated around 340–400 nm with a peak at 370 nm (Chen et al., 1984; Bennett et al., 1994). Recent work by Aidala et al. (2012) also showed that both the Shining Cuckoo (Chalcites [Chrysococcyx] lucidus) and the Long-tailed Cuckoo (Urodynamis [Eudynamis] taitensis) in New Zealand are predicted to possess the short wavelength-sensitive type 1 (SWS1) opsins with maximal sensitivity in the human-visible violet portion of the short-wavelength light spectrum, and not in the UV. Therefore, the UV curves for the three egg phenotypes in cuckoo and its parrotbill host should be regarded as well matching.

    The likelihood of nest predation was not significantly different between nests with white and blue egg in the parrotbill (Yang et al., 2010a). Furthermore, other Paradoxornis species that have no known history of interaction with the cuckoo lay monomorphic eggs in blue color (Jiang et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2011). Given that the cuckoo ancestrally had egg colors that were neither white nor blue (Davies, 2000), it was reasonable to conclude that nest predation is not responsible for the evolution of egg polymorphism in the parrotbill, and selection on the cuckoo for countering the evolution of multiple parrotbill egg types was evidenced by hosts generally having evolved good abilities to reject even partly mimetic eggs (Yang et al., 2010a).

    However, we found that mimicry of blue cuckoo eggs is better than that of white cuckoo eggs in their corresponding host clutches, implying that the white morph may potentially be a secondary egg morph that has not yet evolved fine mimetic features.

    In conclusion, we have shown evidence from photospectrometry that different egg color morphs in the Cuckoo have evolved in response to selection against poor mimics imposed by parrotbill hosts. This evidence supports the hypothesis that the white egg morph in the cuckoo-parrotbill system might be a secondary phenotype that has evolved under the strong selection pressure of brood parasitism.

    We are grateful to Anders P. Møller for valuable comments that significantly improved the quality of the manuscript. We thank Eivin Røskaft, Bård G. Stokke and one anonymous reviewer for helpful comments on our manuscript. This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Nos. 31071938 and 31272328 to WL, 31101646 and 31260514 to CY), Program for New Century Excellent Talents in University (NCET-10-0111 to WL), and Key Project of Chinese Ministry of Education (No. 212136 to CY). We thank the Forestry Department of Guizhou Province and Kuankuoshui National Nature Reserves for support and permission to carry out this study, and J. Wu, X. Guo, X. Xu, N. Wang and L. Wang for assistance with field work.

  • Almond, W.E., 1946. Notes on the song of the Black-headed Bunting. Brit. Birds 39, 315.
    Andersson, M.B., 1994. Sexual Selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton.
    Ballentine, B., Badyaev, A., Hill, G.E., 2003. Changes in song complexity correspond to periods of female fertility in blue grosbeaks (Guiraca caerulea). Ethology 109, 55-66.
    Balsby, T.J.S., 2000. The function of song in Whitethroats Sylvia communis. Bioacoustics 11, 17-30.
    Balsby, T.J.S., Dabelsteen, T., 2003. Male singing behaviour and female presence in the territory in whitethroats Sylvia communis. Acta Ethol. 5, 81-88.
    Barišić, S., Ćiković, D., Tutiš, V., Kralj, J., Hoi, H., 2018. Song structure and variability in the Black-headed Bunting (Emberiza melanocephala). J. Ornithol. 159, 389-400.
    Barton, K., 2019. R package MuMIn: multi-model inference. . (Accessed 11 January 2021).
    Birkhead, T.R., 1991. Sperm depletion in the Bengalese finch, Lonchura striata. Behav. Ecol. 2, 267-275.
    Birkhead, T.R., 1998. Sperm competition in birds: mechanisms and function. In: Birkhead, T.R., Møller, A.P. (Eds.), Sperm Competition and Sexual Selection. Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 579-622.
    Birkhead, T.R., Møller, A.R., 1992. Sperm Competition in Birds: Evolutionary Causes and Consequences. Academic Press, London.
    Birkhead, T.R., Møller, A.P., 1993a. Sexual selection and the temporal separation of reproductive events: sperm storage data from reptiles, birds and mammals. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 50, 295-311.
    Birkhead, T.R., Møller, A.P., 1993b. Why do male birds stop copulating while their partners are still fertile? Anim. Behav. 45, 105-118.
    Birkhead, T.R., Atkin, L., Møller, A.P., 1987. Copulation behaviour of birds. Behaviour 101, 101-138.
    Birkhead, T.R., Cunningham, E.J.A., Cheng, K.M., 1996. The insemination window provides a distorted view of sperm competition in birds. P. Roy. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. 263, 1187-1192.
    Blem, C.R., 1976. Patterns of lipid storage and utilization in birds. Am. Zool. 16, 671-684.
    Burnham, K.P., Anderson, D.R., 2004. Model Selection and Multimodel Inference. Springer, New York.
    Byers, J., Hebets, E., Podos, J., 2010. Female mate choice based upon male motor performance. Anim. Behav. 79, 771-778.
    Candolin, U., 2003. The use of multiple cues in mate choice. Biol. Rev. 78, 575-595.
    Catchpole, C.K., Slater, P.J.B., 2008. Bird song: biological themes and variations. Cambridge University Press, New York.
    Christie, P.J., Mennill, D.J., Ratcliffe, L.M., 2004. Chickadee song structure is individually distinctive over long broadcast distances. Behaviour 141, 101-124.
    Ćiković, D., Barišić, S., Hahn, S., Tutiš, V., Kralj, J., Briedis, M., 2021. Tracking migration of black-headed buntings Emberiza melanocephala reveals the Iranian Plateau as an ecological barrier along the Indo-European flyway. J. Avian Biol. 52, e02783.
    Cramp, S., Perrins, C.M., 1994. The Birds of the Western Palearctic Vol Ⅸ., Handbook of the Birds of Europe, the Middle East and North Africa: the Birds of the Western Palearctic. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
    De Benedictis, P., 1966. The flight song display of two taxa of Vermilion Flycatcher, Genus Pyrocephalus. Condor 68, 306-307.
    Emlen, J., 1957. Display and mate selection in the Whydahs and Bishop Birds. Ostrich 28, 202-213.
    Haftorn, S., 1989. Seasonal and diurnal body weight variations in titmice, based on analyses of individual birds. Wilson Bull. 101, 217-235.
    Halupka, K., Borowiec, M., 2006. Male whitethroats, Sylvia communis, advertise their future contribution to parental care. Behaviour 143, 1-14.
    Hedenstrom, A., Møller, A.P., 1992. Morphological adaptations to song flight in passerine birds: a comparative study. P. Roy. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. 247, 183-187.
    Hyndman, R.J., Athanasopoulos, G., 2018. Forecasting: Principles and Practice. OTexts, Melbourne.
    Hyndman, R.J., Khandakar, Y., 2008. Automatic time series forecasting: the forecast package for R. J. Stat. Soft. 27, 1-22.
    Iwasa, Y., Pomiankowski, A., Nee, S., 1991. The evolution of costly mate preferences II. The "handicap" principle. Evolution 45, 1431-1442.
    Jacobs, A., Zuk, M., 2010. Parasites and sexual selection. In: Jacobs, A. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior. Academic Press, Oxford, pp. 636-641.
    Kempenaers, B., Verheyen, G.R., Dhondt, A.A., 1995. Mate guarding and copulation behaviour in monogamous and polygynous blue tits: do males follow a best-of-a-bad-job strategy? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 36, 33-42.
    Klit, I., 1999. The function of song forms in the lesser whitethroat Sylvia curruca. Bioacoustics 10, 31-45.
    Krams, I., 2002. Mass-dependent take-off ability in wintering great tits (Parus major): comparison of top-ranked adult males and subordinate juvenile females. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 51, 345-349.
    Lang, M., 2011. Der Singflug des Ortolans Emberiza hortulana. Vogelwelt 132, 17-33.
    Liu, W-C., Kroodsma, D.E., Yasukawa, K., 2007. Dawn and daytime singing behavior of chipping sparrows (Spizella passerina). Auk 124, 44-52.
    Máthér, M.H., Robertson, R.J., 1992. Honest advertisement in flight displays of Bobolinks (Dolichonyx oryzivorus). Auk 109, 869-873.
    Merilä, J., Sorjonen, J., 1994. Seasonal and diurnal patterns of singing and song-flight activity in Bluethroats (Luscinia svecica). Auk 111, 556-562.
    Metcalfe, A.V., Cowpertwait, P.S.P., 2009. Introductory Time Series with R. Springer, New York.
    Møller, A.P., Pomiankowski, A., 1993. Why have birds got multiple sexual ornaments? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 32, 167-176.
    Moran, I.G., Lukianchuk, K.C., Doucet, S.M., Newman, A.E.M., Williams, H., Norris, D.R., et al., 2019. Diel and seasonal patterns of variation in the singing behaviour of Savannah Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis). Avian Res. 10, 26.
    Pennycuick, C.J., 1989. Bird Flight Performance: a Practical Calculation Manual. Oxford University Press, New York.
    Pereyra, M.E., Morton, M.L., 2010. Flight songs of Dusky Flycatchers: a response to bird-hunting raptors? J. Field Ornithol. 81, 42-48.
    R Core Team, 2020. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. . (Accessed 28 November 2020).
    Ritchison, G., 1991. The flight songs of common yellowthroats: description and causation. Condor 93, 12-18.
    Rokitansky, G., 1969. Beobachtungen an einem türkischen Kappenammer Emberiza melanocephala Scop. Gefied. Welt 93, 68-69.
    Sheldon, B.C., Burke, T., 1994. Copulation behavior and paternity in the chaffinch. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 34, 149-156.
    Simmons, R., 1988. Honest advertising, sexual selection, courtship displays, and body condition of polygynous male harriers. Auk 105, 303-307.
    Sorjonen, J., Merila, J., 2000. Response of male Bluethroats Luscinia svecica to song playback: evidence of territorial function of song and song flights. Ornis Fenn. 77, 43-47.
    Specht, R., 2014. Avisoft-SASLab Pro. . (Accessed 8 July 2017).
    Taff, C.C., Patricelli, G.L., Freeman-Gallant, C.R., 2014. Fluctuations in neighbourhood fertility generate variable signalling effort. P. Roy. Soc. B. Biol. Sci. 281, 20141974.
    Usherwood, J.R., 2008. Collared doves Streptopelia decaocto display with high, near-maximal muscle powers, but at low energetic cost. J. Avian Biol. 39, 19-23.
    Vignal, C., Máthévon, N., Mottin, S., 2008. Mate recognition by female zebra finch: analysis of individuality in male call and first investigations on female decoding process. Behav. Process. 77, 191-198.
    Walther, B.A., Clayton, D.H., 2005. Elaborate ornaments are costly to maintain: evidence for high maintenance handicaps. Behav. Ecol. 16, 89-95.
    Westneat, D.F., McGraw, L.A., Fraterrigo, J.M., Birkhead, T.R., Fletcher, F., 1998. Patterns of courtship behavior and ejaculate characteristics in male red-winged blackbirds. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 43, 161-171.
    Witter, M.S., Cuthill, I.C., 1993. The ecological costs of avian fat storage. Philos. T. Roy. Soc. B. 340, 73-92.
    Zuk, M., Kolluru, G.R., 1998. Exploitation of sexual signals by predators and parasitoids. Q. Rev. Biol. 73, 415-438.
  • Related Articles

Catalog

    Figures(8)  /  Tables(5)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (88) PDF downloads (8) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return