Scott ROBINSON, Wendy SCHELSKY, Jeffrey HOOVER. 2013: Does landscape structure contribute to the long-term maintenance of generalized parasitism in cowbirds?. Avian Research, 4(1): 31-38. DOI: 10.5122/cbirds.2013.0005
Citation: Scott ROBINSON, Wendy SCHELSKY, Jeffrey HOOVER. 2013: Does landscape structure contribute to the long-term maintenance of generalized parasitism in cowbirds?. Avian Research, 4(1): 31-38. DOI: 10.5122/cbirds.2013.0005

Does landscape structure contribute to the long-term maintenance of generalized parasitism in cowbirds?

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Scott Robinson, E-mail: srobinson@flmnh.ufl.edu

  • Received Date: 24 Dec 2012
  • Accepted Date: 23 Feb 2013
  • Available Online: 23 Apr 2023
  • Unlike most brood parasites, several species of cowbird (Molothrus) are generalists that parasitize multiple host species across their range and within the same communities; likewise, there is little evidence that individuals within a population specialize on host species. This situation has variously been attributed to the recency of cowbird evolution (the "evolutionary lag" hypothesis) or to hidden costs of rejection by hosts (the "equilibrium" hypothesis). Both hypotheses have some support as cowbirds are indeed a relatively young clade compared with more specialized cuckoos and cowbirds are capable of sophisticated behaviors such as retaliation against rejection ("mafia" behaviors) that would select for acceptance of cowbird eggs. Nevertheless, many species in the Americas have evolved specialized defenses against cowbird parasitism, almost all of which live in more open habitats (e. g., grasslands, shrublands, riparian strips), which indicates that coevolutionary processes can operate in ways that select for host defenses in spite of cowbird counterdefenses. We propose that the structure of landscapes in North America may explain why forest-nesting birds lack defenses against parasitism and reinforce the long-term maintenance of generalized brood parasitism in cowbirds. Because cowbirds require open habitats in which to feed, they are rare or absent in large forest tracts, which dominate much of the forest cover of the Americas. These tracts act as "source habitats" that produce surplus young that recolonize populations in smaller, fragmented forest patches in which rates of both cowbird parasitism and nest predation are very high ("sink" habitats). Evolution of antiparasite adaptations would be very slow in this situation because most hosts are produced in areas where there is little or no cowbird parasitism. In addition, the interplay of host breeding dispersal, source-sink metapopulation dynamics, and fragmented forest habitat could further deter the evolution of host defenses against parasitism. Therefore, as long as large forest tracts remain widespread in North America, most forest birds will likely continue to lack defenses against cowbird parasitism, guaranteeing a steady supply of naïve hosts in forest habitats, even in fragmented landscapes. This situation will, in turn, favor host generalist cowbirds that actively avoid more open habitats in favor of parasitizing forest bird communities. These forest communities may also act as source populations for cowbirds, which might pump surplus generalist cowbirds into more open habitats further slowing the coevolutionary process. As long as large forest tracts are a common part of the landscape, generalist parasitism may persist indefinitely.

  • Birdsong, i.e., learned vocalisation of oscine passerine birds, belongs to the most complex and diverse vocal signals in the animal kingdom and plays crucial roles in species-specific recognition, mate attraction and repelling of competitors (Todt and Naguib, 2000; Catchpole and Slater, 2014). It is a well-established and intensively studied example of animal social learning and cultural evolution (Lynch, 1996; Aplin, 2019) and, thanks to high diversity of songbirds and their ecologies, it exhibits outstanding inter-as well as intraspecific variation (e.g., Slater and Ince, 1979; Podos et al., 2004; Lachlan et al., 2018).

    To achieve species-specific songs, young birds typically memorise songs heard from conspecifics shortly after hatching; subsequently, they practise and perform songs with high plasticity to finally reach their adult crystallised songs (Brainard and Doupe, 2002; Hultsch and Todt, 2004). Although this generalised process seems straightforward, song learning strategies in songbird species are very diverse—from precise imitation of tutors, through improvisation to invention (reviewed in Beecher and Brenowitz, 2005). As such, the accumulation of small variations in song characteristics commonly results in geographic variation of song structures or repertoires (Podos and Warren, 2007). Accordingly, spatial variation in song was documented in many different songbird species, regardless of the complexity of their song (Jenkins, 1977; Osiejuk et al., 2003; Roach and Phillmore, 2017; Barišić et al., 2018; Czocherová et al., 2022).

    However, changes in songs within the same population may also occur over time. Studies comparing recordings obtained after a decade or more reported a whole range of different patterns, from remarkable stability (e.g., Ficken and Popp, 1995; Goodale and Podos, 2010; Ivanitskii et al., 2023) through partial turnovers (e.g., Chilton and Lein, 1996; Nelson et al., 2004; Luther and Baptista, 2010) to a complete lack of similarity between old and more recent recordings from the same area (e.g., Holland et al., 1996). Such changes of song at a single locality might have in return important consequences in the signalling functions. Multiple experimental studies documented that the changes in song could influence the behaviour of males (e.g., Thompson and Baker, 1993; Nelson and Soha, 2004; Szymkowiak and Kuczyński, 2017), females (e.g., Gentner and Hulse, 2000; Spencer et al., 2005; Caro et al., 2010), or both sexes (e.g., Catchpole et al., 1986; Balaban, 1988; Searcy et al., 1997; Anderson et al., 2007). From an evolutionary perspective, female preference for a local variant could, over time, constitute a behavioural barrier to gene flow via assortative mating and eventually favour a process of speciation (Derryberry, 2007).

    Moreover, long-term studies of song variation focusing on a single population could provide a good background to explore the mechanisms behind song evolution, such as learning biases, selection on signal efficiency, or cultural drift (e.g., Williams and Lachlan, 2022). For example, a study on Chestnut-side Warblers (Dendroica pensylvanica) revealed a rapid change in one song type but almost none in another one, suggesting that multiple independent cultural traditions and probably multiple independent learning predispositions can evolve concurrently within a species (Byers et al., 2010). Similarly, the three parts of the song of Savannah Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis), likely serving different functions, apparently undergo changes at different rates and may be affected by different evolutionary mechanisms, both neutral and selective (Williams et al., 2013).

    Most of the studies focusing on long-term temporal song evolution have so far examined species with rather simple songs (e.g., Gibbs, 1990; Grant and Grant, 1996; Hansen, 1999; Byers et al., 2010; Pipek et al., 2016). Recently, however, song repertoire evolution after almost four decades was documented in a population of the House Finch (Haemorhous mexicanus), a species with moderately complex song, showing that the song types had undergone a complete turnover while remaining structurally similar (Ju et al., 2019). Nonetheless, the opposite has been documented on a larger scale in Common Nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos). In this species with very large song repertoires (over 190 song types per individual; Hultsch and Todt, 1981; Kipper et al., 2004), a remarkable stability of some of the most common song types was shown after four to nine decades (Jäckel et al., 2022).

    In this study, we investigated the song evolution of the Thrush Nightingale (Luscinia luscinia), a sister species to the Common Nightingale. It also has a relatively large repertoire (though much smaller than the latter species) and complex syntax (Grießmann and Naguib, 2002; Ivanitskii et al., 2017), and shows a pronounced geographical song variation (Sorjonen, 1987; Souriau et al., 2019). We compared songs recorded at the same locality in Finland 33 years apart (in 1986 and 2019), focusing on both syllable and song type levels. We hypothesised that song and syllable type sharing would be overall lower between old and recent recordings than between recordings from the same year, and that syllables, as basic units of vocalisation, are more likely to persist over decades than song types but that at least some song types will be recorded in both years.

    The Thrush Nightingale (Luscinia luscinia; Passeriformes: Muscicapidae) is a small migratory songbird breeding in the northern part of Eurasia and wintering in Southeast Africa (Cramp and Perrins, 1988; Collar, 2020). Males are highly philopatric (Sorjonen, 1987) and defend their territories by singing during both night and day (Sorjonen, 1977). As usual for small passerines, the males usually start breeding in the next year after hatching, and while the longest documented lifespans are nine to ten years (Cramp and Perrins, 1988; Glutz von Blotzheim, 1988), most individuals probably live much shorter. Natural song rate of territorial males usually ranges between six to eight songs per minute (Vokurková et al., 2013; Souriau et al., 2018, 2021), with individual songs separated by several-second-long pauses.

    When characterising Thrush Nightingale song features in this study, we followed the general approach described in Souriau et al. (2019): a "syllable" is the smallest invariant distinct unit within a song consisting of one or several "elements" (i.e., continuous traces on the spectrogram) that are always repeated in the same sequential order. Individual songs of Thrush Nightingales, ca 5 s long, usually begin with a sequence of various initial syllables that is highly variable even among songs of the same individual, and end with a characteristic sequence of two consecutive phrases: loud "castanet" syllables followed by a deep "rattling" broadband trill (Fig. 1; Sorjonen, 1983; Souriau et al., 2019), often followed by a short terminal syllable. For the purposes of this study, we defined the "song type" as a set of songs that share a distinct conserved sequence of several syllables (or their repetitions) in the final part of the song (Fig. 1), usually covering both castanet and rattling phrases, i.e., structures that are typically used to characterise song types in this species (Grießmann and Naguib, 2002; Ivanitskii et al., 2017).

    Figure  1.  Spectrograms displaying three instances of the song types most frequently used in each study year (left: song type SF05 recorded in 1986, right: SF01 recorded in 2019). Each spectrogram shows a song sung by a different individual (indicated in the upper left corner) and displays various degrees of divergence in syllable use, especially high in the initial parts of the song. The ending part of the song that defines these two song types, consisting of the characteristic loud low-pitched castanet and broadband rattling, is highlighted by a horizonal bar. Male codes of males recorded in 1986 start with A (as "ancient"), those of 2019 start with R ("recent"). The catalogue of all song types recognised within this study is provided along with other data in the Zenodo repository (doi: 10.5281/zenodo.14196640).

    As a consequence of the song learning process, syllable as well as song type repertoires are shared by Thrush Nightingale males at the same location. For example, reported proportions of shared song types reached 30% on the Hiddensee island in the Baltic Sea (Grießmann and Naguib, 2002) and 47% in Moscow, Russia (Ivanitskii et al., 2017).

    Song recordings were obtained in two breeding seasons, 1986 and 2019, in the region of Parikkala, south-eastern Finland, in an area surrounding the Siikalahti pond (radius of ca 6.5 km around 61.564° N, 29.560° E). The males were singing in semi-open habitats, from shrubs along roads and fields to denser vegetation and woodland edges, usually close to pond or lakes (Fig. 2). In this area, Thrush Nightingale population dynamics has been studied by J.S. in detail in the early 1980s, with proportion of returning individuals between years ranging from 9% to 40% (Sorjonen, 1987).

    Figure  2.  Study area of Parikkala, south-eastern Finland (star in the inset map), in the vicinity of Siikalahti pond and surrounding wetlands. Red dots with male codes indicate the origin of the analysed recordings from 2019. The exact origin of the recordings from 1986 is not known (due to the unavailability of geolocation devices at that time), but all were recorded in the same area by one of the authors (J.S.).

    All males at the locality were recorded only once, in long uninterrupted recordings that lasted 11–32 min in 2019 and 15–25 min in 1986. Songs were recorded during short spring nights, between 20:20 and 05:45, from a distance of ca 10 m, close enough to maximise the recording signal to noise ratio but far enough not to disturb or interrupt the male’s singing activity. Analogue recordings in 1986 were obtained by J.S. using Uher 4400 Report IC and Uher M 517 microphones along with a JVC TL-E71 parabolic sound reflector, and were digitised at Luomus Helsinki Museum, Finland to 16bit PCM mono format with 96 kHz sampling frequency. For the purpose of our study, they were downsampled to 44.1 kHz. Recordings from 2019 were obtained by A.S. using an Olympus LS-5 handheld recorder with high sensitivity and low-cut filter settings, and saved in PCM format (16 bit, mono, 44.1 kHz).

    The recording period lasted over one month in 1986 (mid-May to mid-June) and three days (May 30 to June 1) in 2019. As neighbouring males may converge in song use during the same breeding season (Sorjonen, 1987), the timing of the recording might have to some extent influenced the song type repertoires. However, we assume that the different duration of the recording periods should not have affected the qualitative differences (syllable and song type sharing patterns) between the two recording years substantially, especially considering the overall higher song type repertoire sizes in 1986 recordings, regardless of the recording date within the season (see Results, and Appendix Table S1).

    Ten out of 17 males recorded in 2019 were selected for subsequent song analyses, based on the singing context (absence of direct territorial interaction with other males), and on the quality and duration of the recordings (low background noise such as wind, and at least 9 min of continuous singing of the focal male). Recordings of 11 males made in 1986 by J.S. were selected from sound archives based on the same criteria. Precise locations of those recordings were not available; however, they all originated from the same area as the recent ones. The selected recordings were cut to the duration of ca 10 min (keeping the last song in the sequence complete) for the subsequent processing. Details about the analysed recordings and key summary statistics are provided in Appendix Table S1.

    Visualisation and analyses of spectrograms were performed with Avisoft-SASLab Pro 5.2.09 (Specht, 2014) with the following set of parameters: fast Fourier transform (FFT) length 512, frame size 100%, Hamming window, overlap 75%. For each individual, we analysed the truncated 10-min-long section of the recording, which yielded in total 1529 songs (1986: 818, 2019: 711) with a median value of 74 songs per recording (minimum 50, maximum 105).

    All songs were analysed for syllable composition and assigned to song types by the same person (A.S.). Sonograms were checked visually and distinct elements or stable combinations of distinct elements were assigned to a particular syllable type. Certain elements, such as broadband "clicks", could have been classified in different categories, either as a separate syllable in the trill sequences, or as a part of more complex syllables in consistent association with other elements. We also considered syllable syntactic use within the song; syllables of a similar structure slightly differing in frequency characteristics were thus considered as belonging to the same syllable type if used in the same context in a particular song type (see Fig. 3).

    Figure  3.  Spectrograms illustrating syllable diversity in the studied Thrush Nightingale repertoires. The male code is provided above each spectrogram, the syllable type on the left. The first line illustrates how the same syllable type may vary within a sequence. The second line shows examples of inter-individual variation of syllables assigned to the same syllable types (due to similar syllable structure and the same syntactic context in the respective song types) but varying to some extent between each other. The last line compares examples of syllable types found in both 1986 and 2019 recordings, possibly maintained in the local Thrush Nightingale population over more than 30 years. The catalogue of all syllables recognised within this study is provided along with other data in the Zenodo repository (doi: 10.5281/zenodo.14196640).

    To assess how well the composition of individual repertoires was captured, we constructed accumulation curves in which the total number of detected song types was plotted against the number of processed songs within the analysed recording. We used the same approach to create accumulation curves for syllable types; however, to reduce the workload, this was only done for a randomly selected subset of males (five from 1986 and five from 2019).

    To estimate the syllable and song type repertoire sharing between individuals, we calculated the Jaccard similarity coefficient (as in Grießmann and Naguib, 2002), which ranges from 0 (no sharing) to 1 (all syllable or song types, respectively, shared between two individuals). Its complement, Jaccard distance, was used to visualise the song and syllable repertoire similarities between males and years in dendrograms, constructed by unweighted pair group method with arithmetic mean (UPGMA).

    A two-sided non-parametric Mann-Whitney U test was used to evaluate the differences in syllable and song type repertoire sizes, and the extent of within-year repertoire sharing (based on mean value of Jaccard coefficient for each individual). A linear mixed-effect model (lmer function of the lmerTest R package; Kuznetsova et al., 2017), with the male identity as a random intercept, was used to test for differences in the song duration between the two years. The relationship between the geographic distances between individuals recorded in 2019 and their song and syllable repertoire dissimilarity (i.e., the Jaccard distance) was evaluated using a two-sided Mantel test (implemented in the ape package v. 5.0; Paradis and Schliep, 2019) permuted 10,000 times. All statistical analyses were performed with R software version 4.4.1 (R Core Team, 2024) along with the package ggplot2 (Wickham, 2016) for the visual exploration and figure plotting.

    Data and song recordings underlying the present study are provided in the Zenodo repository (https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.14196640) or in the collections of the Luomus Helsinki Museum, Finland. The data in the online repository include all information available about the focal males (which are also provided in Appendix Table S1) and the analysed songs, catalogues of song types and syllables, and 10-min segments of recordings used in the study in a PCM format (sampling frequency 44.1 kHz, 16 bit, mono). Complete song recording from both 1986 and 2019 are available upon request from the Luomus Helsinki Museum.

    The 1529 detected songs were classified into 31 song types (1986: 18, 2019: 13; none shared between the two years) composed of 122 syllable types (1986: 79, 2019: 73; 32 of those shared). The assessment of the relationship between the number of detected song and syllable types, respectively, and the number of analysed songs (Fig. 4) suggested that our sampling duration of 10 min was sufficient. For all analysed individuals, the accumulation curves reached a plateau well before the end of the given recording.

    Figure  4.  (A) Percentage of detected song types from the male’s complete repertoire based on the 10 min recordings from 1986 (n = 11) and 2019 (n = 10). (B) Percentage of detected syllable types from the same recordings (based on five individuals from each year). Half of the birds from both years reached the whole detected repertoire of either song types or syllable types already before the 30th analysed song.

    Repertoires of males recorded in 1986 comprised significantly more song types than those recorded in 2019 (median and range: 8 (7–10) vs. 6 (4–8); Mann-Whitney U test, n1 = 11, n2 = 10, W = 103, p < 0.001), and the same was found for syllable repertoires (median and range: 45 (39–61) vs. 38.5 (28–51); Mann-Whitney U test, W = 90, p = 0.014). Additionally, songs were significantly shorter in 1986 than in 2019 (mean ± SD: 5.0 ± 0.77 s vs. 6.1 ± 1.22 s; linear mixed model: difference = 1.09 s, SE = 0.44, t = 2.49, df = 18.9, p = 0.022; Appendix Fig. S1).

    None of the song types recorded in 1986 was captured in recordings from 2019, though song type sharing within each studied year, expressed as pairwise Jaccard similarity values, was quite high (Fig. 5A and 6A; mean ± SD: J1986 = 0.65 ± 0.30, J2019 = 0.56 ± 0.16). Despite such song type turnover, the species’ characteristic song structure with typical ending was present in both years (Fig. 1), and over 40% of syllable types recorded in 1986 persisted until 2019 (Fig. 5B and 6B; J1986 = 0.72 ± 0.18, J2019 = 0.65 ± 10, J1986–2019 = 0.14 ± 0.03). The syllable sharing with any bird recorded in the same year was always higher than with any bird from the other recording year (Fig. 5B). The within-year sharing was more prominent in 1986 than in 2019 at both song type and syllable level (Figs. 5 and 6; Mann-Whitney U test, n1 = 11, n2 = 10, W = 25, p < 0.04 in both tests).

    Figure  5.  Song type sharing (A) and syllable sharing (B) between each pair of individuals from both old recording (1986: A) and recent ones (2019: R). The number of song or syllable types shared between individuals and the resulting Jaccard similarity coefficient scores are shown above and below the diagonals, respectively. The gradient of colours highlights scores from low (red) to high (green) value.
    Figure  6.  UPGMA dendrogram based on the Jaccard distances calculated from the song type (A) and syllable type (B) repertoires of individuals representing the old (1986: A) and recent (2019: R) populations.

    Despite the overall consistency in repertoires found within each recorded year, clusters of individuals with more similar vocalisation were also identified: this is well illustrated by the high similarity in both syllable and song types between males A-07 and A-11 recorded in 1986, which were distinct from the other 1986 males (Fig. 6). Although their song repertoires contained the three most common song types shared by the whole population (SF03, SF05 and SF13), these were the only two males not singing three other very common song types of that period (SF02, SF07 and SF26) while including in their repertoires three other song types unique to them (SF15, SF16, SF17; Appendix Fig. S2). However, increased similarity of song type repertoires was not related to the spatial distance between the recording locations in the recent population (Mantel test; p = 0.50, Z = 62,799). Indeed, males with most similar repertoires (e.g., R-01, R-09 and R-05, or R-02 and R-06) were not recorded at nearby territories (Fig. 2).

    The songs of Thrush Nightingale males, recorded at the same locality in Finland, changed significantly over the course of 33 years. While almost half of the syllable types persisted, no song type was shared between the two years. Three decades represent a long time period for local song evolution in a small passerine species with short lifespan such as the Thrush Nightingale (Cramp and Perrins, 1988), and many new generations of birds replaced those recorded in 1986. Apart from temporal changes in repertoire composition, we also observed that individual song type as well as syllable repertoires were significantly smaller, and songs were longer in 2019 compared to 1986.

    Despite a limited sample size of our study (11 and 10 males, respectively), the observed patterns were very consistent. As only two points in time were studied, it is not possible to differentiate between the alternative scenarios of the local song evolution, a slow successive modification of the existing song types on the one hand, or rapid "invasion" of new song types from a distant population on the other hand. These two scenarios, however, are not mutually exclusive, as both processes could have affected the local population’s repertoires.

    The most noticeable change occurred at the song type level and was documented despite our inclusive approach, tolerating for high component variability and defining song types based on composition of the most stable song structures, usually by the combination of ending castanet and rattle sequences (Fig. 1; Sorjonen, 1987; Souriau et al., 2019). These results may seem contradictory with those from the study of Jäckel et al. (2022) on Common Nightingales, who showed that part of the most common song types from 1960 persisted till 2019. That study was much larger than ours in both geographic scale (14 countries) and size of the dataset (around 14,000 songs vs. our ca 1500). However, the specifics of Common Nightingale vocalisation may have played a key role in this contrast of the two congeneric species: despite the sharing of the song types among neighbours and some tendency to adjust repertoires to the local population (Hultsch and Todt, 1981; Kiefer et al., 2010), Common Nightingales show a remarkable lack of geographic song variation (Jäckel et al., 2022), and their repertoire sizes (over 190 song types per male; Hultsch and Todt, 1981) are substantially higher than in Thrush Nightingales. On the contrary, Thrush Nightingale males show a pronounced variation of song between locations (Sorjonen, 1987; Souriau et al., 2019) and despite substantial variation of their song, the reported song type repertoires are smaller: 4–10 (conservatively defined) song types recognised in our study, 16–30 in Grießmann and Naguib (2002), 7–23 in Ivanitskii et al. (2017). The persistence of a small fraction (1.2%) of the most common songs found in Common Nightingales (Jäckel et al., 2022), might then not be observable in the smaller repertoire of Thrush Nightingales.

    In contrast to a complete turnover of song types, over 40% of syllable types of the 1986 syllable repertoire of Thrush Nightingales at our study locality were still present in 2019. From a syntactical point of view, the smaller song units—syllable types—should be easier to maintain than complex song types formed by their combinations. This suggestion is supported by a small-scale study comparing songs of seven territorial Thrush Nightingale males from an island population within a single breeding season, who generally shared 80% of their song elements but only 30% of their song types (Grießmann and Naguib, 2002).

    While the variation of Thrush Nightingale song over time is well documented, consequences of such changes still need to be investigated. Both neutral processes, such as cultural drift and population fluctuations, and adaptive responses to the local environmental conditions or social contexts, may shape males’ repertoires. An interesting feature of Thrush Nightingale behaviour, highlighting its plasticity in vocalisation, is the tendency of the males to copy the songs of its closest related species, the Common Nightingale, in regions of their sympatry in a process known as "mixed singing" (Sorjonen, 1986; Vokurková et al., 2013). This prominent change in species’ song repertoire upon contact with its congener is considered adaptive in interspecific territorial interactions (Reif et al., 2015; Souriau et al., 2018). The capability to adapt to each season’s song type repertoire of competitors (Sorjonen, 1987; Grießmann and Naguib, 2002) is likely beneficial in intraspecific interactions of Thrush Nightingale males. Such behaviour, if persisting over a long period, together with occasional immigration of new individuals bringing new song variants, could thus eventually lead to a complete replacement of song types at the local population level.

    A complete turnover of song types has also been observed in other songbird species with varying song complexity. For example, a population of the Hermit Warbler (Setophaga occidentalis) apparently changed its simple song over a decade only, with just a few remnant individuals singing the old song type (Janes and Ryker, 2013). Rapid repertoire change was also recently shown on a very large scale in the North American White-throated Sparrow (Zonotrichia albicollis), in which a characteristic dialect song ending spread from a local to a continental scale in just two decades (Otter et al., 2020). Temporal dynamics of song structures comparable to ours was also reported for the House Finch, a species of moderate song diversity, from a locality considered to be the species’ historical introduction site (Ju et al., 2019). After almost four decades, a complete turnover of song type repertoires occurred without any major change in song structures, and half of the syllable types still remained shared between the studied years. Such diversification of songs between individuals over time may have been caused by a rapid population expansion following a cultural founder effect (Ju et al., 2019).

    The studied Thrush Nightingale population at our locality was well established and the potential influence of founder effects can be ruled out. We suggest that local population dynamics is the more likely reason for the observed changes in song. The Thrush Nightingale density in 2019 was apparently lower in comparison with 1986 (J.S., pers. observation). Lower density might result in less interactions between males, which therefore could result in longer songs, as observed in 2019, with a lower risk of interruption by counter-singing males. Similarly, interacting with fewer individuals might result in lower song type sharing and subsequently lead to smaller repertoires. Similar patterns were documented in a study on White-crowned Sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys oriantha) from populations of different size and territorial connectivity after 26 years (Harbison et al., 1999). In a large population, the song structure remained highly consistent, only accumulating small copying errors of syllables. In contrast, smaller populations from fragmented habitats showed an important loss of syllables and the introduction of new ones brought by individuals from nearby populations. The same was observed in a 40-year study on three endangered species of Hawaiian honeycreepers (Fringillidae: Drepanidinae) where a loss of song diversity and complexity followed substantial population declines (Paxton et al., 2019).

    Such a dramatic event has also been reported from the Thrush Nightingale population at our study site at Siikalahti, Finland, when variation of song repertoires between 1980 and 1985 were compared (Sorjonen, 1987). The overall male’s tendency to converge in song repertoires was interrupted in 1984, likely due to a substantially higher rate of immigration compared to the other years, and more than half of the song types changed over a single year (Sorjonen, 1987). Similarly, the displacement of a Thrush Nightingale population due to unusual climatic conditions in the Moscow region, Russia, resulted in the invasion of "southern" song types into the northern population’s repertoire (Marova et al., 2015), and abrupt changes in song repertoires have also been described at the meta-population scale in the same region over a decade (Ivanitskii et al., 2022).

    Species’ vocalisation may also adapt to changes of the surrounding environment, such as vegetation density (Tubaro et al., 1993; Derryberry, 2009) or anthropogenic noise levels (Slabbekoorn and Peet, 2003; Derryberry, 2009; Cardoso and Atwell, 2010; Luther and Baptista, 2010). Changes in response to increased anthropogenic noise have also been reported for the Thrush Nightingales (Ivanitskii et al., 2015). Although our study area has experienced woody plant encroachment over open and semi-open habitats over the recent years (J.S., pers. observations), the males themselves were found in similar habitats, preferred by the species, in both 1986 and 2019. Furthermore, if the change of vegetation structure drove repertoire composition of the studied Thrush Nightingale populations, we would expect that trills, the song structures particularly sensitive to acoustic degradation in close habitats (Sorjonen, 1983), would be particularly affected. However, the same syllables in both fast and slow trill types were used frequently in 1986 as well as in 2019. Therefore, we are convinced that adaptations to habitat changes were not responsible for the observed changes of Thrush Nightingale vocalisation between the study years.

    To conclude, our analysis across a period of more than three decades revealed a substantial change in population repertoires, with a complete turnover of song types but only a partial change in syllable repertoires. The processes responsible for the observed song evolution, and whether the changes took place gradually or abruptly, cannot be assessed from this study, which was limited by sample size and two time points only. Interestingly, the song type repertoires of Thrush Nightingales turned to be less stable in our study area after three decades than was the case for its congener with substantially larger repertoire, the Common Nightingale, after almost seven decades (Jäckel et al., 2022). More studies on long term song variation in a broad number of songbird species, with different song complexity and learning mechanisms, are needed to better understand the processes and mechanisms affecting the cultural evolution of song and their outcomes.

    Abel Souriau: Writing – original draft, Visualization, Methodology, Investigation, Formal analysis, Data curation, Conceptualization. Jorma Sorjonen: Writing – review & editing, Resources, Investigation, Conceptualization. Adam Petrusek: Writing – review & editing, Visualization, Validation, Methodology, Formal analysis, Conceptualization. Tereza Petrusková: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Supervision, Methodology, Formal analysis, Conceptualization.

    The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

    We are grateful to Risto Väinölä and Jari Valkama, as well as the Finnish Museum of Natural History for making this collaborative project possible. We thank Reetta Vairimaa for bringing the light on the two sides of our ornithological work, and her help with connecting two generations of nightingale scientists over a short and prolific field season in the beautiful lake region of Eastern Finland. Pavel Linhart, Silke Kipper and anonymous referees provided useful insights during various phases of the study. As always, we extend our thanks to all the helpful colleagues and friends for their insightful comments and everyday support, and especially the Museum’s Bat Lab team for their warm hospitality.

    Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avrs.2025.100224.

  • Brawn JD, Robinson SK. 1996. Source-sink dynamics may complicate the interpretation of long-term census data. Ecology, 77: 3–12.
    Briskie JV, Sealy SG, Hobson KA. 1992. Behavioral defenses against avian brood parasitism in sympatric and allopatric host populations. Evolution, 46: 334–340.
    Cavitt JF, Martin TE. 2002. Effects of forest fragmentation on brood parasitism and nest predation in eastern and western landscapes. Stud Avian Biol, 25: 73–80.
    Chace JF, Farmer C, Winfree R, Curson DR, Jense WE, Goguen CB, Robinson SK. 2005. Cowbird ecology: a review of factors influencing distribution and abundance of cowbirds across spatial scales. Orn Monogr, 57: 45–70.
    Colbeck GJ, Gibbs HL, Marra PP, Hobson K, Webster MS. 2008. Phylogeography of a widespread North American migratory songbird (Setophaga ruticilla). J Hered, 99: 453–463.
    Davies NB. 2000. Cuckoos, Cowbirds and Other Cheats. T & AD Poyser, London.
    Delcourt PA, Delcourt H. 1987. Long Term Forest Dynamics: A Case Study of Late-Quaternary Forests in Eastern North America. Ecological Studies 63. Springer-Verlag, New York.
    Donovan TM, Lamberson RH, Kimber A, Thompson Ⅲ FR, Faaborg J. 1995. Modeling the effects of habitat fragmentation on source and sink demography of neotropical migrant birds. Cons Biol, 9: 1396–1407.
    Fauth PT. 2001. Wood thrush populations are not all sinks in the agricultural Midwestern United States. Conserv Biol, 15: 523–527.
    Friesen L, Cadman MD, Mackay RJ. 1999. Nesting success of neotropical migrant songbirds in a highly fragmented landscape. Conserv Biol, 13: 338–346.
    Goguen CB, Mathews NE. 2001. Brown-headed cowbird behavior and movements in relation to livestock grazing. Ecol Appl, 11: 1533–1544.
    Gomulkiewicz R, Thompson JN, Holt RD, Nuismer SL, Hochberg, ME. 2000. Hot spots, cold spots, and the geographic mosaic theory of coevolution. Am Nat, 156: 156–174.
    Grim T. 2002. Why is mimicry in cuckoo eggs sometimes so poor? J Avian Biol, 33: 302–305.
    Hahn DC, Hatfield JS. 1995. Parasitism at the landscape scale: cowbirds prefer forests. Conserv Biol, 9: 1415–1424.
    Hauber ME, Yeh PJ, Roberts JOL. 2004. Patterns and coevolutionary consequences of repeated brood parasitism. Proc R Soc Lond B, 271: S317–S320.
    Holt RD. 1996. Adaptive evolution in source-sink environments: direct and indirect effects of density-dependence on niche evolution. Oikos, 75: 182–192.
    Holt RD, Gomulkiewicz R. 1997. How does immigration influence local adaptation: A reexamination of a familiar paradigm. Am Nat, 149: 563–572.
    Hoover JP. 2003a. Decision rules for site fidelity in a migratory bird, the prothonotary warbler. Ecology, 84: 416–430.
    Hoover JP. 2003b. Experiments and observations of prothonotary warblers indicate a lack of adaptive responses to brood parasitism. Anim Behav, 65: 935–944.
    Hoover JP, Brittingham MC. 1993. Regional variation in cowbird parasitism of wood thrushes. Wilson Bull, 105: 228–238.
    Hoover JP, Hauber ME. 2007. Individual patterns of habitat and nest-site use by hosts promote transgenerational transmission of avian brood parasitism status. J Anim Ecol, 76: 1208–1214.
    Hoover JP, Reetz MJ. 2006. Brood parasitism increases provisioning rate, and reduces offspring recruitment and adult return rates, in a cowbird host. Oecologia, 149: 165–173.
    Hoover JP, Robinson SK. 2007. Retaliatory mafia behavior in a parasitic cowbird favors host acceptance of parasitic eggs. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 104: 4479–4483.
    Hoover JP, Brittingham MC, Goodrich LJ. 1995. Effects of forest patch size on nesting success of wood thrushes. Auk, 112: 146–155.
    Hoover JP, Tear TH, Baltz M. 2006a. Edge effects reduce the nesting success of Acadian Flycatchers in a moderately fragmented forest. J Field Ornith, 77: 425–436.
    Hoover JP, Yasukawa K, Hauber ME. 2006b. Spatially and temporally structured avian brood parasitism affects the fitness benefits of hosts' rejection strategies. Anim Behav, 72: 881–890.
    Lawes MJ, Marthews TR. 2003. When will rejection of parasite nestlings by hosts of nonevicting avian brood parasites be favored? A misimprinting-equilibrium model. Behav Ecol, 14: 757–770.
    Lindholm AK, Thomas RJ. 2000. Differences between populations of reed warblers in defences against brood parasitism. Behaviour, 137: 25–42.
    Lotem A, Nakamura H, Zahavi A. 1992. Rejection of cuckoo eggs in relation to host age: a possible evolutionary equilibrium. Behav Ecol, 3: 128–132.
    Lotem A, Nakamura H, Zahavi A. 1995. Constraints on egg discrimination and cuckoo-host co-evolution. Anim Behav, 49: 1185–1209.
    Martínez JG, Soler JJ, Soler M, Møller AP, Burke T. 1999. Comparative population structure and gene flow of a brood parasite, the Great Spotted Cuckoo (Clamator glandarius), and its primary host, the Magpie (Pica pica). Evolution, 53: 269–278.
    Moskát C, Bán M, Székely T, Komdeur J, Lucassen RWG, van Boheemen LA, Hauber ME. 2010. Discordancy or template-based recognition? Dissecting the cognitive basis of the rejection of foreign eggs in hosts of avian brood parasites. J Exp Biol, 213: 1976–1983.
    Pulliam HR. 1988. Sources, sinks, and population regulation. Am Nat, 132: 652–661.
    Ripple WJ, Van Valkenburgh B. 2010. Linking top-down forces to the Pleistocene megafaunal extinctions. Bioscience, 60: 516–526.
    Robinson SK, Hoover JP. 2011. Does forest fragmentation and loss generate sources, sinks, and ecological traps in migratory songbirds? In: Liu J, Hull V, Morzillo A, Wiens JA (eds) Sources, sinks and Sustainability across Landscapes. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 423–449.
    Robinson SK, Thompson FR Ⅲ, Donovan TM, Whitehead DR, Faaborg J. 1995a. Regional fragmentation and the nesting success of migratory birds. Science, 267: 1987–1990.
    Robinson SK, Rothstein SI, Brittingham MC, Petit LJ, Grzybowski JA. 1995b. Ecology and behavior of cowbirds and their impact on host populations. In: Martin TE, Finch DM (eds) Ecology and Management of Neotropical Migratory Birds. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 428–460.
    Robinson SK, Brawn JD, Morse SF, Herkert JR. 1999. Use of different habitats by breeding brown-headed Cowbirds in fragmented midwestern landscapes. Stud Avian Biol, 18: 52–61.
    Robinson SK, Hoover JP, Herkert JR. 2000. Cowbird parasitism in a fragmented landscape: Effects of tract size, habitat, and abundance of cowbirds and hosts. In: Cook T, Robinson SK, Rothstein SI, Sealy SG, Smith JNM (eds) Ecology and Management of Cowbirds. University of Texas Press, Austin, Texas, pp 280–297.
    Røskaft E, Moksnes A, Stokke BG, Moskát C, Honza M. 2002. The spatial habitat structure of host populations explains the pattern of rejection behavior in hosts and parasitic adaptations in cuckoos. Behav Ecol, 13: 163–168.
    Røskaft E, Takasu F, Moksnes, Stokke B. 2006. Importance of spatial habitat structure on establishment of host defenses against avian brood parasitism. Behav Ecol, 17: 700–708.
    Rothstein SI. 1974. Mechanisms of avian egg recognition: possible learned and innate factors. Auk, 91: 796–807.
    Rothstein SI. 1978. Mechanisms of avian egg recognition: additional evidence for learned components. Anim Behav, 26: 671–677.
    Rothstein SI, Robinson SK. 1998. The evolution and ecology of avian brood parasitism: an overview. In: Rothstein SI, Robinson SK (eds) Parasitic Birds and Their Hosts: Studies in Coevolution. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 3–56.
    Soler JJ, Martínez JG, Soler M, Møller AP. 1999. Genetic and geographic variation in rejection behavior of cuckoo eggs by European Magpie populations: an experimental test of rejectergene flow. Evolution, 53: 947–956.
    Strausberger BM, Ashley MV. 2005. Host use strategies of individual female Brown-headed Cowbirds (Molothrus ater) in a diverse avian community. J Avian Biol, 36: 313–321.
    Steadman DW, Martin PS. 1984. Extinction of birds in the late Pleistocene of North America. In: Martin PS, Klein RG (eds) Quaternary Extinctions. University of Arizona Press, Tucson, pp 466–477.
    Teuschl Y, Taborsky B, Taborsky M. 1998. How do cuckoos find their hosts? The role of habitat imprinting. Anim Behav, 56: 1425–1433.
    Tewksbury JJ, Hejl SJ, Martin TE. 1998. Breeding productivity does not decline with area in a western landscape. Ecology, 79: 2890–2903.
    Thompson JN. 1999. Specific hypotheses on the geographic mosaic of coevolution. Am Nat, 153: S1–S14.
    Trine CL. 1998. Wood Thrush population sinks and implications for the scale of regional conservation strategies. Conserv Biol, 12: 576–585.
    Veit ML, Robertson RJ, Hamel PB, Friesen VL. 2005. Population genetic structure and dispersal across a fragmented landscape in Cerulean Warbler (Dendroica cerulean). Conserv Gen, 6: 159–174.
    Ward MP, Schlossburg S. 2004. Conspecific attraction and the conservation of territorial songbirds. Conserv Biol, 18: 519–524.
    Winfree R, Dushoff J, Robinson SK, Bengali D. 2006. A Monte Carlo model for estimating the productivity of a generalist brood parasite across multiple host species. Evol Ecol Res, 8: 213–236.
    Woolfenden BE, Gibbs HL, Sealy SG, McMaster DG. 2003. Host use and fecundity of individual female Brown-headed Cowbirds. Anim Behav, 66: 95–106.
  • Related Articles

Catalog

    Article Metrics

    Article views (1675) PDF downloads (1344) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return