Andrew M. Allen, Bruno J. Ens, Martijn van de Pol, Henk van der Jeugd, Magali Frauendorf, Henk-Jan van der Kolk, Kees Oosterbeek, Jeroen Nienhuis, Eelke Jongejans. 2019: Colour-ring wear and loss effects in citizen science mark-resighting studies. Avian Research, 10(1): 11. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-019-0151-z
Citation: Andrew M. Allen, Bruno J. Ens, Martijn van de Pol, Henk van der Jeugd, Magali Frauendorf, Henk-Jan van der Kolk, Kees Oosterbeek, Jeroen Nienhuis, Eelke Jongejans. 2019: Colour-ring wear and loss effects in citizen science mark-resighting studies. Avian Research, 10(1): 11. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-019-0151-z

Colour-ring wear and loss effects in citizen science mark-resighting studies

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Andrew M. Allen, Andrew.Allen@science.ru.nl

  • Received Date: 16 Aug 2018
  • Accepted Date: 07 Apr 2019
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 14 Apr 2019
  • Background 

    Ring wear and loss may have important consequences for mark-recapture studies that aim to estimate survival trends. Our study quantifies the rates of wear and loss from a long-running colour-ringing project of the Eurasian Oystercatcher (Haematopus ostralegus) in the Netherlands.

    Methods 

    Our analysis included 8909 colour-ringed oystercatchers with 118,071 resightings,predominantly ringed and observed by citizen scientists. We quantified how ring wear and loss may vary with ring colour and age,and how this may impact resighting probabilities. We then determined how ring loss may impact survival estimates and resighting probabilities of mark-recapture models by simulating four scenarios of how citizen scientists may resight and report birds with lost colour rings.

    Results 

    Annual rates of colour ring loss averaged 2.5% compared with 1% for ring wear,but lost rings also had a higher reporting probability (31.2%) than worn rings (10.3%). Lost rings may not directly impact survival estimates since 50% of oystercatchers with lost rings could still be uniquely identified. Ring wear and loss rapidly increased between 10 and 15 years after ringing. Rates of ring loss were comparable amongst ring colours,but the wear rate appeared higher for red and white rings compared to other colours. Rates of ring wear and loss varied significantly amongst different regions in our study,which were characterised as having different habitat features. Differential rates of ring wear may have important implications for studies conducted over large geographical scales or of multiple species.

    Conclusions 

    Based on simulations of ring loss in our population,estimates of apparent survival may be 0.3-1.2% lower whilst the impact of ring wear was deemed even lower. We developed a table of recommendations containing advice for ring fitting,data collection,study design,and mark-recapture analyses,so that future colour-ringing studies can benefit from our experiences in collecting and analysing data of ring wear and loss through citizen science.

  • Avian brood parasitism studies have provided us with some of the best examples of co-evolved adaptations (Rothstein, 1990; Davies, 2000). Obligate brood parasites receive parental care from unrelated individuals (hosts) of other species. Successful parasitism often reduces host reproductive output, which favors the evolution of host defenses, leading to selection for more sophisticated trickeries by the parasite to overcome host defenses (Rothstein, 1990). This escalation of adaptations and counter-adaptations by both sides leads to a coevolutionary "arms race" between parasites and hosts (Davies, 2000).

    Discrimination and rejection of parasitic eggs are the most widespread and efficient host defensive mechanisms specifically evolved to counteract the costs imposed by brood parasitism (Rothstein, 1990; Payne, look like and recognize and reject those with an appearance different from that of their own eggs (Victoria, 1972; Rothstein, 1975; Lotem et al., 1992). Experimental studies have shown that hosts of avian brood parasites are more likely to reject parasitic eggs that look different from their own eggs (e.g. Brooke and Davies, 1988; Davies and Brooke, 1988; Soler and Møller, 1990; Moksnes et al., 1993; Lotem et al., 1995; Marchetti, 2000; Moskát and Honza, 2002; Lathi and Lathi, 2002; Rutila et al, 2002; Cassey et al., 2008; Avilés, 2008; Avilés et al., 2010; Spottiswoode and Stevens, 2010; Stoddard and Stevens, 2010), which supports the view that mimetic cuckoo egg phenotypes have evolved due to host egg rejection.

    As matching between parasite and host eggs improves, hosts are expected to reduce variation in the appearance of their eggs within their clutches. A low variation in egg appearance within host clutches would facilitate host's recognition of parasite eggs (e.g. Victoria, 1972; Davies and Brooke, 1989b). In addition, a high homogeneity in appearance within clutches would lead to a higher variation in egg appearance among clutches of different host individuals (i.e. inter-clutch variation). Hence, a higher inter-clutch variation in egg appearance will prevent the evolution of a good match for the parasite (Soler and Møller, 1996). Passerine species frequently parasitized by the Common Cuckoo (Cuculus canorus) have evolved a lower degree of intraclutch variation in egg appearance, and a higher degree of inter-clutch variation than those that have not experienced such interactions (e.g. Øien et al., 1995; Soler and Møller, 1996; Stokke et al., 2002; Avilés and Møller, 2003). Recent findings with three Australian hosts of the Pallid Cuckoo (Cuculus pallidus), however, cast doubts on the generality of these processes as intra-clutch variation did not vary between populations with different exposure to parasitism (Landstrom et al., 2011). Intra-specific evidence has so far focused on reporting a link between intra-clutch variation in host egg appearance and probability of rejection of artificial eggs, and, is even more controversial. While some studies have shown that low intra-clutch variation in host egg appearance may favor parasite egg discrimination (Stokke et al., 1999; Soler et al., 2000; Moskát et al., 2008), others did not find support for the parasite recognition function of such egg traits (Karcza et al., 2003; Avilés et al., 2004; Honza et al., 2004; Stokke et al., 2004; Lovászi and Moskát, 2004; Polacikova et al., 2007; Cherry et al., 2007; Landstrom et al., 2010). This variability in results may reflect differences between host species in rejection abilities as the role of intra-clutch variation must be presumably low in good host rejecters as compared to moderate ones (Stokke et al 2007). However, still disparate results might arise due to age-related rejection and egg appearance (e.g. Lotem et al., 1995), and/or a genetic link between egg appearance and rejection behavior (Moskat et al., 2008), that can only be disentangled by experimental manipulations. Indeed, Moskát et al. (2008), showed strong experimental support for the intra-clutch hypothesis as they reported a reduced discrimination of foreign eggs as a consequence of an experimental increase of egg phenotypes variation in a cuckoo host, the Great Reed Warbler (Acrocephalus arundinaceus).

    The Azure-Winged Magpie (Cyanopica cyanus) is a social small corvid with a remarkable disjunct distribution. It occurs widely in the eastern Palaearctic and is also found in a smaller geographical area in Spain and Portugal (Cramp and Perrins, 1994). Parasitism of the Azure-winged Magpie by the Common Cuckoo has become very frequent in some parts of the eastern Palearctic (Nakamura et al., 1998). In the Iberian Peninsula two obligate brood parasite species, namely the Great Spotted Cuckoo (Clamator glandarius) and the Common Cuckoo, might potentially exploit the Azurewinged Magpie as host since they are largely sympatric in many areas (Cramp and Perrins, 1994). Although evidence of Common Cuckoo parasitism in Iberia is restricted to one locality in Central Spain (Alonso et al. 1991), evidence coming from egg collections kept in museums suggests that Great Spotted Cuckoo parasitism was widespread at the end of the 19th century because at least 11 cases of parasitized clutches by the Great Spotted Cuckoo were reported in different localities of Spain and Portugal (Avilés, 2004). Indeed, previous studies have shown that Azure-winged Magpies have remarkable discrimination abilities, readily rejecting Great Spotted Cuckoo models and eggs (Arias de Reyna and Hidalgo, 1982; Arias de Reyna, 1998; Avilés, 2004). Also, experiments with adult cuckoo mounts have shown that Azure-winged Magpies defended their nests more aggressively (i.e. higher intensity and shorter latency to an aggressive response) towards Great Spotted Cuckoo and predator dummies than towards nonharmful control species (Avilés and Parejo, 2006).

    Here we experimentally parasitized Azure-winged Magpie nests with one of three types of parasite eggs gradually differing in matching from host eggs. In agreement with the arms race hypothesis we predicted: 1) that those individuals with a lower within-clutch variation in egg appearance should be more prone to reject parasite eggs; and, 2) that the role of low intraclutch variation in parasite egg discrimination increased with greater mimicry between parasite and host eggs.

    The field study was carried out in the surroundings of Guadiloba reservoir near Cáceres (37°18'N, 3°11'W), southern Spain during March–June 2003–2004. The study area approximately comprised 400 ha of a wooded plot of holm oak trees (Quercus ilex) (termed dehesa). Azure-winged Magpie nests were mostly found in holm oak trees.

    The Azure-winged Magpie is a flexible cooperative breeder with occurrence of helper individuals assisting the dominant pair at different stages of the breeding cycle (Valencia et al., 2003). Azure-winged Magpies usually lay five or six eggs (range 2–8) and most first clutches in the study area are found in April and the first week of May (J.M. Avilés, unpublished). Only females incubate the eggs, starting some days after the first egg is laid (Komeda et al., 1987). Egg coloration and maculation varies among females from pale cream or olive-cream backgrounds, rarely with bluish tinge, to pale yellowish-brown or brown-olive, with sparse small brown or olive-brown spots and greyish blotches, often concentrated at the broad end (Cramp and Perrins 1994; Fig. 1). Individual females have a consistent egg type, and based on human assessment their intra-clutch variation is in general very low (Fig. 1). Great Spotted Cuckoo eggs are elliptical to sub-elliptical, with blunt ends, smooth and fairly glossy, pale green-blue, thickly spotted, light brown or red brown color (Cramp and Perrins 1994; Fig. 1) and they are not good mimics to Azure-winged Magpie eggs according to the human eye (Fig. 1). Also spectrophotometric analyses of Azurewinged Magpie clutches parasitized by Great Spotted Cuckoos saved in European egg collections revealed a poor level of mimicry between cuckoo and Azurewinged Magpie host eggs (Soler et al., 2003).

    Figure  1.  Variation in egg appearance within clutches of Azurewinged Magpie (a, b). Representative differences in appearance between Azure-winged Magpie eggs (five top eggs) and experimental Azure-winged Magpie egg in the bottom (c); Representative appearance of model Great Spotted Cuckoo egg (d), and real Great Spotted Cuckoo eggs (e) used in the experiments. Mean dimensions of experimental eggs were: model Great Spotted Cuckoo eggs = 32.5 mm×25.1 mm; real Great Spotted Cuckoo eggs = 31.4 mm×23.8 mm, Azure-winged Magpie eggs = 26.7 mm×20.0 mm.

    We searched for nests at the beginning of the breeding seasons 2003–2004. 76 nests were found at different stages of building and were numbered and mapped. The nests were visited daily and each egg was numbered with waterproof ink in consecutive order as laid. We did not detect evidence of intraspecific parasitism since only one egg ever appeared on a single day. Eight Azure-winged Magpie nests were predated before responses to artificial parasitism could be assessed. The remaining 68 nests were randomly assigned to one of the following groups: Experimentally parasitized with Great Spotted Cuckoo model eggs (n = 19), experimentally parasitized with real Great Spotted Cuckoo eggs (n = 18) or experimentally parasitized with conspecific eggs (n = 31). Size dimensions of eggs in each of these groups are reported in the Fig. 1. The three experimental groups were not equally distributed between the two years of study. Experiments with Great Spotted Cuckoo model eggs were performed in 2003, while response to real Great Spotted Cuckoo eggs was assessed in 2004 only. However, parasitism with conspecific eggs was performed in 2003 (n = 14) and 2004 (n = 17). Since the study was undertaken for 2 seasons, and birds were not banded, there is a possibility that the same individuals have been included in two experiments.

    Great Spotted Cuckoo model eggs were quail (Coturnix japonica) eggs painted with acrylic paint to mimic the color and spotted pattern of real cuckoo eggs in the area (Avilés, 2004, Fig. 1). Real Great Spotted Cuckoo eggs were collected from deserted Magpie (Pica pica) nests. We did not quantify variation between cuckoo eggs in our population, but they all showed a considerably degree of mismatching with host eggs according to the human eye (Fig. 1). Conspecific eggs were collected from recently abandoned nests in the study area. We avoided using conspecific eggs that were abandoned more than a week before artificial parasitism because eggs may fade in color due to storage (Moreno et al., 2011). Experimental eggs were not reused and no host eggs were removed during experimental parasitism to simulate Great Spotted Cuckoo laying tactics and conspecific parasitism (Avilés, 2004).

    Dimensions of Great Spotted Cuckoo models were within the range size and mass of real Great Spotted Cuckoo eggs measured in the area, but slightly larger than the average (Fig. 1). In addition, although models were painted to match the color and pattern of a typical cuckoo egg in the area, it should be noted that to the human eye real Great Spotted Cuckoo eggs achieved a better matching of host eggs than our models (Fig. 1). Hence, matching in egg size and appearance between host and parasite eggs greatly varied among the three experimental treatments ranging from poor (cuckoo models) to good (conspecific eggs), with real Great Spotted Cuckoo eggs being at an intermediate stage (Fig. 1).

    The time interval between laying of the first host egg and the beginning of the experiment varied. 47.0% of nests (n = 32) were artificially parasitised during host egg laying period, and the remaining 53.0% (n = 36) were parasitised 2–3 days after completion of the clutch. However, no breeding phase effect on rejection is expected since response to artificial parasitism in the same Azure-winged Magpie population did not vary between laying and incubation (Avilés, 2004). In no case did artificial parasitism cause host eggs to be removed the subsequent days. All nests were checked every second day and response to parasitism was assessed six days from artificial parasitism as rejection (eggs ejected or nest deserted) or acceptance of parasitic eggs (eggs still warm and incubated in nest).

    Experimental eggs were always added to Azure-winged Magpie nests after the laying of the fifth egg, which is about one day in advance to clutch completion. Thus, in all experimental nests, hosts were able to assess intraclutch variation of their entire clutch before experiencing artificial parasitism. At that time we obtained two different measures of intra-clutch variation in host eggs based on human assessment: one accounting for variation in color and spottiness estimated from photographs, and the second yielding variation in egg size. Because we took photos one day before clutch completion we were not able to assess the total intra-clutch variation (at least one egg laid afterwards). However, the last egg laid is not different in appearance from the others in Azure-winged Magpies (J.M. Avilés, pers. obs.) making it unlikely that the appearance of the last egg may affect our results. Host clutches were photographed in a standardized way with a Nikon Coolpix 990 digital camera. Clutches were numbered and photographed on a standard Kodak neutral-grey card. Intra-clutch variation in egg color and marking was later assessed on a 1–5 scale (1 = no variation, 5 = all the eggs differ from each other) (Øien et al., 1995) by five experienced ornithologists that did not know the fate of the nest in advance and that were unaware of the hypothesis being tested. Consistency of the five scores was significant (repeatability = 0.51, F19, 80 = 6.28, p < 0.0001), and one mean score value per clutch as an estimation of intraclutch variation in egg color and marking was then calculated. In addition, length and width of all eggs were taken using a caliper with a precision of 0.01 mm. Egg volume was later estimated from the formula V = 0.515LB2 (Hoyt, 1979), and the standard deviation in egg volume within a clutch was used in further analyses as a measure of intra-clutch variation in egg size.

    We are aware that our method of estimating variation in egg appearance was based upon human vision and hence does not account for UV information, which birds might perceive (e.g. Cherry and Bennett, 2001; Avilés and Møller, 2003; Avilés et al., 2006). However, human assessments of intra-clutch variation have the advantage of incorporating patterns of speckling of the eggs, which are not easily tractable by using spectrophotometry.

    We performed logistic regression models with the GENMOD procedure of SAS (SAS Institute, 1996) to test for the association between the occurrence of rejection (rejection vs. acceptance of parasitism) and the two measures of intra-clutch variation and the type of experimental egg introduced into the Azure-winged Magpie nests. Probability of rejection was modeled as a binomial response variable (1 = rejection, 0 = acceptance) using a logistic link function. To account for the fact that the three experimental groups were not equally distributed in the two years (see data collection) we introduced the experimental treatment nested in the study year and the study year as a fixed effect in the model.

    Previously, the existence of age-related rejection and egg color features for some cuckoo hosts have been reported (e.g. Lotem et al., 1992). To account for this possibility we tested for the associations between rejection of artificial eggs and clutch size that is assumed an age-dependent feature among passerine birds (e.g. Lack, 1968). Furthermore, we tested for the possible association between variation in egg traits and host clutch size by using Pearson correlations (GLM procedure in SAS). Starting models contained all the main effects plus all possible interactions. Model selection was carried out by removing, one by one, the effects that were the furthest to statistical significance, starting with the highest-order interactions down to the main effects.

    Proneness to rejection by Azure-winged Magpies increased with degree of dissimilarity between introduced experimental eggs and host eggs (Table 1): Great Spotted Cuckoo model eggs were more frequently rejected (73.68%) than real Great Spotted Cuckoo eggs (44.44%) and than the more mimetic conspecific eggs (35.5%, Fig. 2). In addition, we found that intra-clutch variation in egg size, but not in egg color and marking, almost significantly related to the probability of accepting or rejecting parasitic eggs by Azure-winged Magpies (Table 1). Individuals with higher variation in egg-size were more prone, although not significantly (p = 0.07), to be rejecters than individuals showing lower variation in egg size (Fig. 3).

    Table  1.  Relationship between rejection of parasitic eggs and the type of parasitic egg (Great Spotted Cuckoo model, Great Spotted Cuckoo and conspecific eggs), intra-clutch variation in egg color and marking (ITCM), intra-clutch variation in egg size (ITS) and year
    χ2 df p
    Egg type×ITCM×ITS 2.19 2, 47 0.33
    ITCM×ITS 2.51 1, 49 0.11
    Egg type×ITCM 4.27 2, 50 0.12
    Egg type×ITS 2.90 2, 52 0.23
    ITCM 2.82 1, 54 0.09
    ITS 3.21 1, 58 0.07
    Egg type 7.40 2, 58 0.025
    Study year 0.13 1, 58 0.72
    Tests performed with generalized linear models with occurrence of rejection (rejection vs. acceptance of parasitism) as a binomial dependent variable and the two measures of intra-clutch variation, the type of experimental egg nested in the study year and the study year as independent variables. Model selection started from the complete model. All effects and interactions are shown irrespective of level of significance.
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV
    Figure  2.  Egg rejection in relation to level of mimicry between the parasite and Azure-winged Magpie eggs. Rejection rate increased with dissimilarity between parasite and host eggs after accounting for unequal distribution of experimental eggs in the two years (χ2 = 7.40, df = 2, 58, p = 0.025).
    Figure  3.  Mean (±S.E.) intra-clutch variation in egg size (i.e., S.D. in volume within cluth) in relation to the response to artificial parasitism (rejection vs. acceptance). Sample sizes are shown close to the mean values.

    We found no support for the existence of age-related responses to parasitism by Azure-winged Magpies. First, the probability of Azure-winged Magpies rejecting model eggs was not significantly associated with clutch size (mean = 5.87, S.D. = 0.18, n = 35 in acceptors vs. mean = 6.09, S.E. = 0.15, n = 32 in rejecters, χ2 = 1.00, df = 1, 65, p = 0.32). Furthermore, neither intraclutch variation in egg color and marking (rp = -0.14, p = 0.28, n = 60), nor intra-clutch variation in egg size (rp = -0.11, p = 0.39, n = 60) were significantly associated to Azure-winged Magpie clutch size.

    Obligate avian brood parasites may greatly reduce host fitness and then actively select for the evolution of antiparasite defensive mechanisms in their hosts. In the coevolutionary arms race between cuckoos and their hosts, discrimination and rejection of parasite eggs by hosts is a main defensive mechanism reducing parasite prevalence and favoring the evolution of parasite egg mimicry (Davies, 2000). Discrimination abilities are then expected to be a function of degree of matching between parasite and host eggs (e.g. Davies, 2000). In addition, it has been suggested that a reduction in intra-clutch variation in egg appearance may help hosts in discriminating against cuckoo eggs highly resembling their ones (e.g. Victoria, 1972; Davies and Brooke, 1989a). As expected, Azure-winged Magpies rejected parasitic eggs in relation to its degree of dissimilarity with their own eggs (Fig. 2, Table 1): nonmimetic Great Spotted Cuckoo model eggs were more frequently rejected than real cuckoo eggs; and these were more frequently rejected than the conspecific eggs. This tendency has been shown by several previous studies on other host species (e.g. Davies and Brooke, 1988, 1989a; Soler and Møller, 1990; Moksnes et al., 1993; Lotem et al., 1995; Lathi and Lathi, 2002; Procházka and Honza, 2003, 2004; Cassey et al., 2008; Avilés et al., 2010), and was suggested to be the factor selecting for mimicry in eggs of the parasite. The trend that we have found would be predicted by either variation in color and marking or variation in size of parasite eggs across the three experimental groups. Therefore, it is difficult to discriminate among degree of matching in color and marking, and in size as the possible factors eliciting rejection of parasitic eggs in the Azure-winged Magpie.

    Our results do not support the first prediction concerning the relationship between intra-clutch variation in egg appearance and discrimination of parasite eggs. We found a non-significant trend for variation in egg size, but not in color or marking, to be important (but p = 0.07) explaining rejection behavior against parasite eggs in the Azure-winged Magpie. Those individuals with a higher intra-clutch variation in egg size within their clutches showed a non-significant trend to reject more frequently than those individuals with a lower intra-clutch variation in egg size (Fig. 3). In addition, our results did not fit the second prediction concerning an increasing role of low intra-clutch variation in parasite discrimination as mimicry between the parasite and the host eggs increases.

    To date a large number of studies using Common Cuckoos or Great Spotted Cuckoos as the target parasite species have assessed the adaptive role of intra-clutch variation in terms of parasite egg discrimination at the intra-specific level (see introduction). Support for this hypothesis came from Stokke et al. (1999), which found the expected negative relationship between intra-clutch variation and rejection probability. Also Moskat et al. (2008) found that when you experimentally altered intra-clutch variation (making it higher than controls), rejection rate decreased in great reed warbler hosts. However, the more prevalent result found in other studies was a no significant relationship between intraclutch variation in egg appearance and parasite egg rejection (Karcza et al., 2003; Avilés et al., 2004; Honza et al., 2004; Stokke et al., 2004; Lovászi and Moskát, 2004; Polacikova et al., 2007; Landstrom et al., 2010). Discrepancies between studies may be due to the different level of recognition abilities for the tested hosts as moderate rejecters might differently rely on intra-clutch variation in egg appearance than good rejecters (Stokke et al., 2007). Also, Polacikova et al. (2007) showed that it may be features at the egg blunt pole rather that those estimated for the whole egg that are important.

    Expectation from the intra-clutch variation hypothesis for the hosts of the Great Spotted Cuckoo should differ from that for hosts of the Common Cuckoo because the former parasite has eggs that do not mimic host eggs (Soler et al., 2003), which would reduce the adaptive value of a low intra-clutch variation in terms of egg discrimination (Victoria, 1972; Davies and Brooke, 1989). Then we may expect intra-clutch variation to be unimportant in terms of discrimination of parasite eggs among Great Spotted Cuckoo hosts. Indeed, results from the three only studies analyzing the hypothesis with hosts of the Great Spotted Cuckoo do not support this prediction. In fact, two studies have found some support for a positive relationship between intra-clutch variation and discrimination abilities (Avilés et al., 2004; this study), whereas the third study found a negative relationship (Soler et al., 2000). This fact may suggest that hosts of Great Spotted Cuckoo, in which parasite-host matching is rather poor (Soler et al., 2003), are using discrimination rules different than the ones assumed for Common Cuckoo hosts when discarding parasite eggs. For instance, it could be advantageous for the hosts to produce eggs with signatures because a foreign egg not bearing such a signature could be easily discriminated. Alternatively, rejection and intra-clutch variation in egg appearance may be age dependent traits in the Azurewinged Magpie. Since egg discrimination by hosts is likely based on a learning mechanism (Victoria, 1972; Lotem et al., 1992), the detected pattern should be a consequence of the greater recognition abilities of parasitic eggs by older Azure-winged Magpies not linked to their egg appearance. However, this explanation seems to be unlikely for this system, because clutch size, that is assumed to be an age-related trait in passerines (e.g. Lack, 1968), was unrelated to rejection rate and to intraclutch variation in egg features (this study).

    Alternatively, our results could be related to other selective pressures acting on azure-winged magpie egg appearance. As well as interspecific brood parasitism, intraspecific brood parasitism and nest predation are factors potentially shaping egg appearance in birds (see review in Underwood and Sealy, 2002). Victoria (1972) predicted that just as in species that are affected by interspecific brood parasitism, a reduction in intra-clutch variability and an increase in inter-clutch variability might increase the chance of discrimination of conspecific eggs when the probability of intraspecific brood parasitism is high. Indeed, Azure-winged Magpies have the potential of discarding conspecific eggs which suggests that egg rejection in this species may have evolved from intra-specific parasitism (Avilés, 2004). However, in long-term studies of the species in Spain (reviewed in Avilés, 2004; Valencia et al., 2004) in which eggs were daily numbered, no evidence of intraspecific brood parasitism was found. Likewise, preliminary studies of parentage of the Azure-winged Magpie revealed no evidence of intraspecific brood parasitism (J.G. Martínez, pers. commun.). On the other hand, nest predation might differently affect individual hosts as a function of the variation in the appearance of their eggs. However, in only 10% of the studies where eggs were artificially placed in nests did changes in egg coloration explain changes in egg predation (Underwood and Sealy, 2002). In addition, we failed to find any link between intraclutch variation in egg color and matching (One-way ANOVA, F1, 28 = 0.16, p = 0.69) and egg size (One-way ANOVA, F1, 28 = 0.11, p = 0.70) and probability of predation while performing our experiments, suggesting a minor role for nest predation on our results.

    In conclusion, our results support the view that Azure-winged Magpies have remarkable cognitive abilities for discriminating against parasite eggs. We found that Azure-winged Magpies rejected parasitic eggs in relation to its degree of dissimilarity with their own eggs. We also found that Azure-winged Magpies that rejected foreign eggs showed an almost significant higher variation in egg size than acceptors, and that the role of intra-clutch variation in parasite egg discrimination does not change with mimicry. The most likely explanation for our results is that Azure-winged Magpie egg coloration may favor recognition of both intra- or inter-specific parasitism because variation in egg appearance was consistently linked to a higher recognition ability of parasite eggs. Considering together ours and previous findings in magpie hosts parasitized by great spotted cuckoos suggesting that a large variation in egg appearance within host clutches may favor parasite egg discrimination, future studies should focus on the study of the plausible discrimination mechanisms leading to such a relationship.

    We thank Carlos de la Cruz for advice on how to search for Azure-winged Magpie nests. Pablo Cidoncha provided invaluable help in the field. We are grateful to the Dirección General de Medio Ambiente de la Junta de Extremadura which provided us with permissions, and to the owners and guards of Casa Urraca and Suerte de Santa Maria who provided us with facilities during field work. We thank two anonymous referees and the guest editors, Eivin Røskaft and Bård Gunnar Stokke that provided constructive comments on the manuscript. This work was performed without financial support.

  • Allen AM, Singh NJ. Linking movement ecology with wildlife management and conservation. Front Ecol Evol. 2016;3:155.
    Allen AM, Ens BJ, van de Pol M, van der Jeugd H, Frauendorf M, Oosterbeek K, Jongejans E. Seasonal survival and migratory connectivity of Eurasian Oystercatchers revealed by citizen science. Auk. 2019;136:1-17. .
    Baccetti N, Morelli F. Which ring colour looks best: results of the Antequera 'Ophthalmic' tests. In: Béchet A, Rendón-Martos M, Amat JA, Baccetti N, Childress B, editors. Proceedings of the IVth international workshop on the greater flamingo in the Mediterranean region and northwest Africa. Flamingo Special Publication 1; 2009. pp. 78-9.
    Baylis SM, Drynan D, Clarke N, Van M, Sunnucks P, Clarke RH. Estimates of wear rates in metal bird bands, with applications for survival and movement models of marked individuals. J Field Ornithol. 2018;89:393-406.
    Bearhop S, Ward RM, Evans PR. Long-term survival rates in colour-ringed shorebirds—practical considerations in the application of mark-recapture models. Bird Study. 2003;50:271-9.
    Berthold P, Bauer HG, Westhead V. Bird migration: a general survey [Internet]. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2001.
    Clark JA, Thorup K, Stroud DA. Quantifying the movement patterns of birds from ring recoveries. Ringing Migr. 2009;24:180-8.
    Clausen KK, Frederiksen M, Madsen J. Measuring neck collar loss of Pink-footed Geese Anser brachyrhynchus. Bird Study. 2015;62:137-40.
    Collins P, Jessop R, Minton C. Fading of colour-bands used on Pied Oystercatchers Haematopus longirostris and Sooty Oystercatchers H. fuliginosus in southeast Australia. Wader Study Gr Bull. 2002;99:50-3.
    Conn PB, Kendall WL, Samuel MD. A general model for the analysis of mark-resight, mark-recapture, and band-recovery data under tag loss. Biometrics. 2004;60:900-9.
    Durell SEALV. Differential survival in adult Eurasian oystercatchers Haematopus ostralegus. J Avian Biol. 2007;38:530-5.
    Duriez O, Sæther SA, Ens BJ, Choquet R, Pradel R, Lambeck RHD, Klaassen M. Estimating survival and movements using both live and dead recoveries: a case study of oystercatchers confronted with habitat change. J Appl Ecol. 2009;46:144-53.
    Duriez O, Ens BJ, Choquet R, Pradel R, Klaassen M. Comparing the seasonal survival of resident and migratory oystercatchers: Carry-over effects of habitat quality and weather conditions. Oikos. 2012;121:862-73.
    Ens BJ, Underhill LG. Synthesis of oystercatcher conservation assessments: general lessons and recommendations. Int Wader Stud. 2014;20:5-22.
    Ens BJ, Kersten M, Brenninkmeijer A, Hulscher JB. Territory quality, parental effort and reproductive success of Oystercatchers (Haematopus ostralegus). J Anim Ecol. 1992;61:703-15.
    Ens BJ, Aarts B, Hallmann C, Oosterbeek K, Sierdsema H, Slaterus R, Troost G, van Turnhout C, Wiersma P, Nienhuis J, van Winden E. Scholeksters in de knel: onderzoek naar de oorzaken van de dramatische achteruitgang van de Scholekster in Nederland. Nijmegen, Netherlands; 2011.
    Harris MP. Loss of weight and legibility of bird rings. Ringing Migr. 1980;3:41-9.
    Juillet C, Choquet R, Gauthier G, Pradel R. A capture-recapture model with double-marking, live and dead encounters, and heterogeneity of reporting due to auxiliary mark loss. J Agric Biol Environ Stat. 2011;16:88-104.
    Kennard JH. Longevity records of North American birds. Bird Band. 1975;46:55-73.
    Koronkiewicz TJ, Paxton EH, Sogge MK. A technique to produce aluminum color bands for avian research. J Field Ornithol. 2005;76:94-7.
    Laake JL. RMark: an R interface for analysis of capture-recapture data with MARK. AFSC Processed Report 2013-01, Seattle; 2013.
    Lyngs P. Wear and loss of rings used on Razorbills Alca torda—further caution in the estimation of large auk survival. Ringing Migr. 2006;23:39-44.
    Meissner W, Bzoma S. Colour rings with individual numbers increase the number of ringing recoveries of small waders. Wader Study Gr Bull. 2011;2:114-7.
    Méndez V, Alves JA, Gill JA, Gunnarsson TG. Patterns and processes in shorebird survival rates: a global review. Ibis. 2018;160:723-41.
    Milligan JL, Davis AK, Altizer SM. Errors associated with using colored leg bands to identify wild birds. J Field Ornithol. 2003;74:111-8.
    Mitchell C, Trinder M. On reading colour rings. Ringing Migr. 2008;24:11-4.
    O'Brien S, Robert B, Tiandry H. Consequences of violating the recapture duration assumption of mark-recapture models: a test using simulated and empirical data from an endangered tortoise population. J Appl Ecol. 2005;42:1096-104.
    Pigniczki C, Kralj J, Volponi S, Žuljević A, Dakhli M-A, Mikuska T, Azafzaf H, Végvári Z. Migration routes and stopover sites of the Eurasian Spoonbill (Platalea leucorodia) between the Carpathian Basin and wintering areas. Ornis Hung. 2016;24:128-49.
    Rees EC, Owen M, Gitay H, Warren S. The fate of plastic leg rings used on geese and swans. Wildfowl. 1990;41:43-52.
    Reynolds TJ, King R, Harwood J, Frederiksen M, Harris MP, Wanless S. Integrated data analysis in the presence of emigration and mark loss. J Agric Biol Environ Stat. 2009;14:411-31.
    Robinson RA, Clark NA, Lanctot R, Nebel S, Harrington B, Clark JA, Gill JA, Meltofte H, Rogers DI, Rogers KG, Ens BJ, Reynolds CM, Ward RM, Piersma T, Atkinson PW. Long term demographic monitoring of wader populations in non-breeding areas. Wader Study Gr Bull. 2005;106(April):17-29.
    Robinson RA, Grantham MJ, Clark JA. Declining rates of ring recovery in British birds. Ringing Migr. 2009;24:266-72.
    Roche EA, Dovichin CM, Arnold TW. Field-readable alphanumeric flags are valuable markers for shorebirds: use of double-marking to identify cases of misidentification. J Field Ornithol. 2014;85:329-38.
    Roodbergen M, van der Werf B, Hötker H. Revealing the contributions of reproduction and survival to the Europe-wide decline in meadow birds: review and meta-analysis. J Ornithol. 2012;153:53-74.
    Runge CA, Martin TG, Possingham HP, Willis SG, Fuller RA. Conserving mobile species. Front Ecol Environ. 2014;12:395-402.
    Rutten AL, Oosterbeek K, Verhulst S, Dingemanse NJ, Ens BJ. Experimental evidence for interference competition in oystercatchers, Haematopus ostralegus. II. Free-living birds. Behav Ecol. 2010;21:1261-70.
    Safriel UN, Harris MP, Brooke MDL, Britton CK. Survival of breeding oystercatchers Haematopus ostralegus. J Anim Ecol. 1984;53:867-77.
    Sandvik H, Erikstad KE, Barrett RT, Yoccoz NG. The effect of climate on adult survival in five species of North Atlantic seabirds. J Anim Ecol. 2005;74:817-31.
    Seber GAF. A review of estimating animal abundance. Biometrics. 1986;42:267-92.
    Senar JC, Carrillo-Ortiz J, Arroyo L. Numbered neck collars for long-distance identification of parakeets. J Field Ornithol. 2012;83:180-5.
    van de Pol M, Bruinzeel LW, Heg D, van der Jeugd HP, Verhulst S. A silver spoon for a golden future: long-term effects of natal origin on fitness prospects of oystercatchers (Haematopus ostralegus). J Anim Ecol. 2006;75:616-26.
    van de Pol M, Vindenes Y, Sæther B-E, Engen S, Ens BJ, Oosterbeek K, Tinbergen JM. Effects of climate change and variability on population dynamics in a long-lived shorebird. Ecology. 2010;91:1192-204.
    van de Pol M, Atkinson PW, Blew J, Crowe O, Delany S, Duriez O, Hötker H, Laursen K, Oosterbeek KH, Petersen A, Thorup O, Tjørve K, Triplet P, Yésou P. A global assessment of the conservation status of the nominate subspecies of Eurasian Oystercatcher Haematopus ostralegus ostralegus. Int Wader Stud. 2014;20:47-61.
    Walker RH, Robinson RA, Leech DI, Moss D, Kew AJ, Barber LJ, Barimore CJ, Blackburn JR, Clewley GD, De Palacio DX, Grantham MJ, Griffin BM, Schäfer S, Clark JA. Bird ringing and nest recording in Britain and Ireland in 2014. Ringing Migr. 2015;30:84-147.
    Ward RM. Darvic colour-rings for shorebird studies: manufacture, application and durability. Int Wader Study Gr Bull. 2000;91:30-4.
    Weiser EL, Lanctot RB, Brown SC, Gates HR, Bentzen RL, Bêty J, Boldenow ML, English WB, Franks SE, Koloski L, Kwon E, Lamarre J-F, Lank DB, Liebezeit JR, McKinnon L, Nol E, Rausch J, Saalfeld ST, Senner NR, Ward DH, Woodard PF, Sandercock BK. Environmental and ecological conditions at Arctic breeding sites have limited effects on true survival rates of adult shorebirds. Auk. 2018;135:29-43.
    White GC, Burnham KP. Program MARK: survival estimation from populations of marked animals. Bird Study. 1999;46:S120-39.
    Wood KA, Nuijten RJM, Newth JL, Haitjema T, Vangeluwe D, Ioannidis P, Harrison AL, Mackenzie C, Hilton GM, Nolet BA, Rees EC. Apparent survival of an Arctic-breeding migratory bird over 44 years of fluctuating population size. Ibis. 2018;160:413-30.
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(14)

    1. Michael J. Campbell, Gabriela F. Mastromonaco, Gary Burness. Interactive Effects of Temperature Acclimation and Dietary Fatty Acids on Metabolic Rate and Body Composition of Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Journal of Experimental Zoology Part A: Ecological and Integrative Physiology, 2025. DOI:10.1002/jez.2915
    2. Hong Jin, Qi Xia, Ziqing Gao, et al. Decreased basal thermogenesis is associated with the downregulation of cellular metabolic activity of organs and serum thyroid hormones in fasting Eurasian Tree Sparrows (Passer montanus). Avian Research, 2025, 16(1): 100220. DOI:10.1016/j.avrs.2024.100220
    3. Pierre Deviche, Karen Sweazea, Nadia Upah. Adjustments to energy provisioning and oxidative balance in response to temperature in a wild passerine. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology, 2025, 305: 111864. DOI:10.1016/j.cbpa.2025.111864
    4. Felix A. Andong, Vincent C. Ejere, Onyinyechukwu A. Agina, et al. Assessment of biometrics and stress indicators of the adult Village Weaver birds (Ploceus cucullatus) during breeding and post-breeding seasons in Jos, Nigeria. Heliyon, 2024, 10(10): e31196. DOI:10.1016/j.heliyon.2024.e31196
    5. Ming Li, Mingru Xu, Jing Wang, et al. Phenotypic flexibility in metabolic adjustments and digestive function in white-shouldered starlings: responses to short-term temperature acclimation. Journal of Experimental Biology, 2024, 227(1) DOI:10.1242/jeb.246214
    6. X. Xu, J. Wang, K. Lv, et al. Thermoregulation and antioxidant defense mechanisms in Eurasian tree sparrows ( Passer montanus ) under acute heat stress. The European Zoological Journal, 2024, 91(1): 406. DOI:10.1080/24750263.2024.2344627
    7. Yujie XuanYuan, Ran Chen, Jieheng Xu, et al. Seasonal acclimatization and temperature acclimation in small passerine birds is achieved via metabolic adjustments. Avian Research, 2023, 14: 100084. DOI:10.1016/j.avrs.2023.100084
    8. Paige Oboikovitz, David L Swanson, Zhi-Yun Jia. Seasonal metabolic flexibility is correlated with microclimate variation in horned larks and house sparrows. Current Zoology, 2022, 68(2): 199. DOI:10.1093/cz/zoab037
    9. Chenxing Li, Chenyu Liu, Pingxia Hu, et al. Seasonal adjustments in body mass and basal thermogenesis in Chinese hwameis (Garrulax canorus): the roles of temperature and photoperiod. Journal of Experimental Biology, 2022, 225(17) DOI:10.1242/jeb.244502
    10. Zhong-Ru Tang, Shen-Yue Chen, Wei Lu, et al. Morphological and physiological correlates of among- individual variation in basal metabolic rate in two passerine birds. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology, 2022, 267: 111160. DOI:10.1016/j.cbpa.2022.111160
    11. Limin Wang, Ghulam Nabi, Li Zhang, et al. Seasonal variations in gonad morphology and hypothalamic GnRH-I and GnIH in Eurasian Tree Sparrow, a multi-brooded passerine. Avian Research, 2022, 13: 100037. DOI:10.1016/j.avrs.2022.100037
    12. David L. Swanson, Yufeng Zhang, Ana Gabriela Jimenez. Skeletal muscle and metabolic flexibility in response to changing energy demands in wild birds. Frontiers in Physiology, 2022, 13 DOI:10.3389/fphys.2022.961392
    13. Ghulam Nabi, Danning Xing, Yanfeng Sun, et al. Coping with extremes: High-altitude sparrows enhance metabolic and thermogenic capacities in the pectoralis muscle and suppress in the liver relative to their lowland counterparts. General and Comparative Endocrinology, 2021, 313: 113890. DOI:10.1016/j.ygcen.2021.113890
    14. Limin Wang, Ghulam Nabi, Liyun Yin, et al. Birds and plastic pollution: recent advances. Avian Research, 2021, 12(1) DOI:10.1186/s40657-021-00293-2

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(4)  /  Tables(6)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (197) PDF downloads (8) Cited by(14)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return