Martin Päckert, Yue-Hua Sun, Balduin S Fischer, Dieter Thomas Tietze, Jochen Martens. 2014: A phylogeographic break and bioacoustic intraspecific differentiation in the Buff-barred Warbler (Phylloscopus pulcher) (Aves: Passeriformes, Phylloscopidae). Avian Research, 5(1): 2. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-014-0002-x
Citation: Martin Päckert, Yue-Hua Sun, Balduin S Fischer, Dieter Thomas Tietze, Jochen Martens. 2014: A phylogeographic break and bioacoustic intraspecific differentiation in the Buff-barred Warbler (Phylloscopus pulcher) (Aves: Passeriformes, Phylloscopidae). Avian Research, 5(1): 2. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-014-0002-x

A phylogeographic break and bioacoustic intraspecific differentiation in the Buff-barred Warbler (Phylloscopus pulcher) (Aves: Passeriformes, Phylloscopidae)

More Information
  • Background 

    According to current taxonomy only three out of 27 Sinohimalayan leaf warbler species (Phylloscopidae) are considered genetically uniform across their entire breeding range along the Southeastern margin of the Qinghai-Tibetan Plateau, the Buff-barred Warbler (Phylloscopus pulcher) being one of them. Because marked differentiation among Himalayan and Chinese populations has been recently demonstrated for a number of Phylloscopus species (or sister species) we investigated the intraspecific variation of a mitochondrial gene, songs and morphology of P. pulcher in a phylogeographic approach.

    Methods 

    We sequenced a fragment of the mitochondrial cytochrome b, reconstructed haplotype networks and analyzed DNA polymorphism among Himalayan and Chinese populations. We measured time and frequency parameters of two distinct song types and analyzed among population-differentiation in a principal component analysis and a discriminant analysis. We also compared measurements of body size dimensions taken from museum specimens.

    Results 

    The mitochondrial haplotype network (cytb) was divided into two distinct clusters corresponding to geographic origin of samples. Pairwise genetic distances among Himalayan and Chinese mtDNA lineages account for 1.3% which coincides with Pleistocene lineage separation at roughly 650, 000 years ago. Genetic diversity is slightly higher in the Chinese part of the specieso range with respect to haplotype and nucleotide diversity while the less diversified Himalayan population lineage shows signs of recent range expansion. The vocal repertoire of P. pulcher comprises two distinct verse types that are combined with short interspersed click notes to long continuous song displays. Trill verse types showed significant differences among regions in almost all measured frequency and time parameters: Chinese males displayed more rapid and more broad-banded trills at a lower pitch. In contrast, warbling verse types showed a distinctively different structure among regions: Himalayan songs consisted of repeated syllables while Chinese songs comprised repetitions of single, long and strongly modulated elements. Subtle morphological differences among specimens from the two study regions could only be confirmed for plumage coloration but not for metric characters.

    Conclusions 

    Based on the genetic and bioacoustic distinctiveness of Chinese Buff-barred Warbler populations, we recommend that the name Phylloscopus pulcher vegetus Bangs, 1913 should be re-validated for this taxon.

  • Habitat loss and fragmentation are of central concern for the conservation of North American forest birds, since declines in their populations have been associated with habitat loss and fragmentation (Peterjohn 2003; Valiela and Martinetto 2007; Pimm and Brooks 2013; Hindmarch et al. 2017). Identifying aspects of habitat change that have the greatest effects on survival, reproduction, and movement of forest birds will be informative to conservation efforts (Zollner and Lima 2005). For example, increased forest edge has been associated with fragmentation and smaller patch size, and it increases the occurrence of nest predation and lower reproductive success, which contributes to bird population declines (Hoover et al. 1995; Chalfoun et al. 2002). In addition, birds traveling through fragmented landscapes suffer an increase in energy expenditure and longer travel times due to their reluctance to cross open areas (Lima 1993; Sieving et al. 1996; Zollner and Lima 2005) and instead prefer to take detours within forest corridors (St. Clair et al. 1998) to avoid an increased predation risk. One of the animal movements that may encounter this reluctance is homing behavior, which refers to "the ability of certain animals to return to a given place when displaced from it, often over great distances." (The Editors of Encyclopaedia Britannica 2017). Non-migratory birds have strong winter fidelity, but it is less well-known for canopy cover effect on homing behavior (Krištín and Kaňuch 2017). This study was undertaken to help further understand the homing behavior of non-migratory birds during a non-breeding season in an urban environment.

    Forest birds are affected in varying degrees by landscape changes depending on their physical and behavioral characteristics (Newbold et al. 2013). Identifying the characteristics of birds most susceptible to habitat change will also aid conservation efforts (Schlaepfer et al. 2002). A generalist species is able to thrive in a wide variety of environmental conditions and can make use of a variety of resources. A specialist species with limited diet can only thrive in a narrow range of environmental conditions (Royle 2012; Begon et al. 2014). Birds that are specialists are more reluctant than generalists to cross habitat gaps during playback experiments (Sieving et al. 1996; Rail et al. 1997) and demonstrate longer homing times after translocations due to increased use of detour routes (Gillies and St. Clair 2008). Individual species also have varying threshold distances at which they are willing to cross gaps (St. Clair et al. 1998) and different perceptual ranges for which they can perceive the fragmented landscape (Lima and Zollner 1996). All of these factors may affect their willingness to, and speed of, travel through fragmented landscapes.

    We undertook this study to understand how canopy cover and species identity in an urbanized area affect birds' homing success and speed in order to contribute information concerning urban green space planning. We had two specific objectives: (1) to characterize variation in non-breeding season homing propensity (success and speed of return) among common bird species, and (2) to test for the effects of canopy cover on the homing success and speed of translocated birds. Similar to previous studies, translocating individual birds away from their home ranges were used to control the motivation to home, the distance traveled, and the percentage of forest cover in the homing path (Bélisle et al. 2001; Blumstein and Fernandez-Juricic 2010). Unlike most other studies, however, translocations were performed during the non-breeding season because homing propensity in this period was of particular interest to us (Odum 1941; Benvenuti and Ioalè 1980). Past studies that conducted homing experiments during the non-breeding season have explored how age group, spatial memory, species identity, sex, and forest type influence avian homing behavior (Benvenuti and Ioalè 1980; Baccetti et al. 1995; Keiser et al. 2005; Krištín and Kaňuch 2017). However, little is known about the forest canopy cover effect on avian homing behavior in an urban environment where urban forests are fragmented by large roads and urban developments. Based on the contrasting biology of the different species and previous work, we made some general predictions: (1) All species (Carolina Chickadee Poecile carolinensis, Tufted Titmouse Baeolophus bicolor and Northern Cardinal Cardinalis cardinalis) should demonstrate some level (at least 30%) of winter homing success because of the tendency toward winter territoriality is a characteristic of passerines (Tompkins 1933; Salomonson and Balda 1977; Holmes et al. 1989; Krištín and Kaňuch 2017), (2) homing speed of all species should increase with an increase in the percentage of forest cover over the homing path (Bélisle et al. 2001), and (3) Paridae species (chickadees and titmice, also refer as parid), as foraging generalists and canopy-foragers, should return faster than Northern Cardinals (ground-foragers), which are thought to be less likely to cross gaps (Sieving and Karr 1997; Miller and Cale 2000; Vergara and Simonetti 2006).

    Six different trap locations were established around Gainesville, Florida, USA (Fig. 1). Four of the sites were on the University of Florida campus. One site was located in a Gainesville neighborhood (4 km NE of campus) and the other was at the USDA/APHIS Florida Field station (6 km east of campus). For each capture location, a minimum of two translocation drop-off sites were utilized to sample a diversity of canopy covers in return paths.

    Figure  1.  Study location and sampling sites. The left map shows all six feeder trap locations within the city of Gainesville, Florida, USA. The right inset for one feeder trap, shows the contrasting translocation paths

    Three study species, Tufted Titmouse, Carolina Chickadee, and Northern Cardinal were captured and translocated. These common bird species are all resident birds and maintain territoriality year-round. Birds were caught using walk-in wire treadle traps on top of platform bird feeders during non-breeding seasons (September‒February) in 2009 and 2011 (same bird feeder were used from study of Hetrick and Sieving 2012). Then birds were translocated from 0.3 to 1.6 km away, with the majority of translocations occurring at 1 km distance. The traps were baited with safflower and black oil sunflower seeds and left open for two weeks prior to trapping. Visitation to feeder traps was monitored during the pre-trapping periods to ensure that birds were frequently visiting the feeders. One feeder trap was established in each of six trapping locations in each year. Once captured, the birds were marked with unique color band combinations. Birds were then kept in individual cloth bags, during transport by car or bicycle, for less than one hour after capture (typically, times between capture and final release were less than 20‒30 min). Birds were released near canopy cover to allow for safe perching immediately after release. The release time were from 8:00 am to 4:00 pm. At the beginning of translocation experiments, 4 short distance translocations (200‒700 m) were performed to examine whether birds would return at all. Later, all translocations were generally longer (1‒1.6 km). The known winter home range sizes of our study species are all less than one kilometer in width (CACH: 532 m, NOCA: 420 m, TUTI: 914 m; Brewer 1961; Condee 1970; Ritchison and Omer 1990). This winter home range size is based on the maximum movement distance between two points within a territory. Thus, our translocation distances ensured that birds were released in unfamiliar territories.

    The percent of canopy cover associated with the different homing paths was determined using Google Earth TM. We used the map image (year 2009) at 4 km eye altitude and overlaid a circle surrounding the translocation path with the same diameter as the straight-line path between the capture and release sites (Visual illustration in Additional file 1: Figure S1). The circular areas were overlaid with a grid containing the same density of squares (incomplete squares are not used). The number of squares within the circular area that had visible tree canopy were counted and divided by the total number of gridded squares to obtain % canopy cover. We used a circular area rather than a straight-line path to better predict the canopy cover of possible homing routes since birds were not tracked during their movement home and were likely to wander from the straight line (Duhl et al. 2012).

    Each feeder trap was equipped with a motion sensitive camera (Audubon Bird Cam TM or Reconyx Rapidfire TM) that displayed the time and date on each picture, which allowed each homing bird to be identified by its unique markings following return to the feeder. We recorded whether an individual successfully returned or not (1, 0) by checking photos of birds at the capture feeder until a maximum of 800 h (33 days) had passed since release at the translocation site. If the individual was not photographed in that time, then it was counted as not returned (0). We utilized the time and date of release at the translocation site and the time and date of the first detection of the bird at the home feeder by the motion sensitive camera to determine the homing speed in hours. This method may underestimate both the number of birds that returned to capture sites, and their homing speeds, due to hesitancy to revisit the feeder where they were captured. However, given that high percentage of birds (66%) clearly revisited the feeder's post-translocation within 1 or 2 days of initial capture, we do not think that the returning proportion is seriously underestimated (given the long grace period we used)—even trap-shy birds would have had time to recover. For more precise return measurement, combinations of cameras, radio-tracking with automated receivers, mist-netting, and systematic observational efforts utilizing territorial and mobbing playbacks at capture locations could enhance precision (Castellón and Sieving 2006; Desrochers et al. 2011; Villard and Haché 2012; Volpe et al. 2014; Jones et al. 2017; Krištín and Kaňuch 2017). Our method minimized impacts on the well-being of the birds (of interest to us) over these other methods, and our methods were standardized across sites and species. In any mark recapture methodology, precision is at issue. While we do not know how well our method performed relative to others, a methodological study of civet capture–recapture found that the use of camera traps with lures did not change animal movement patterns but did increase the number of captures over un-baited camera traps (Gerber et al. 2012). Perhaps, habituating the birds to a food lure prior to translocation insured a high probability of recapture detection post translocation. In any case, standardized methods of detection for three species that habitually visit feeders allowed us to make relative comparisons among species and return path conditions; precise homing frequencies were not the goal.

    Statistical analyses using linear model (LM) and generalized linear model (GLM) were run in R (version 4.0.2). We ran two types of models: (1) to test how canopy cover, translocation distance, species identity, and release time influence homing success of our study species; (2) to test how those predictors influence homing speed of our study species. We used GLM-binomial for the homing success model and LM for the homing speed model. The homing speed variable (hours to return) in LM was log transformed to fit a normal distribution.

    A total of 71 translocations were performed of 3 different species (summarized in Table 1). An average of 66% of all birds were re-sighted at feeders in their home territory or capture site within 33 days. We found that Northern Cardinals were less likely to return than Carolina Chickadees (Coef = 1.847, SE = 0.932, p = 0.048; Table 2) and Tufted Titmice (Coef = 1.718, SE = 0.648, p = 0.008; Table 2). Among individuals returned to the capture sites (n = 44), the percentage of canopy cover did influence homing speed; birds translocated in areas with higher tree canopy cover returned much faster (Coef = ‒ 0.531, SE = 0.196, p = 0.010; Table 2), and Tufted Titmice returned much faster than Northern Cardinals (Coef = ‒ 1.100, SE = 0.440, p = 0.017; Table 2).

    Table  1.  Summary of homing translocations
    Species N % Canopy cover Translocation distance % Return Hours to return
    CACH 11 42 (32‒73) 1096 (1000‒1500) 82 199 (23‒508)
    NOCA 29 55 (26‒73) 1091 (240‒1500) 45 248 (11‒705)
    TUTI 31 50 (24‒73) 1090 (240‒1620) 81 90 (4‒763)
    Summary 71 51 (24‒73) 1092 (240‒1620) 66 154 (4‒763)
    N represents the total number of translocations; % Canopy cover represents the percentage of tree canopy cover for each translocation path; % Return represents the percentage of translocated individuals returned to capture sites. Values of % Canopy cover, Translocation distance, and Hours to return in the table are mean values with the minimum and maximum range in (). CACH represents Carolina Chickadee, NOCA represents Northern Cardinal, and TUTI represents Tufted Titmouse
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV
    Table  2.  Summary of model outputs of homing success model and homing speed model
    Variables Homing success model (GLM) Homing speed model (LM)
    Coef Std. error p value Coef Std. error p value
    Intercept ‒ 0.278 0.416 0.503 4.842 0.361 < 0.001
    Species CACH 1.847 0.932 0.048 ‒ 0.228 0.589 0.700
    Species TUTI 1.718 0.648 0.008 ‒ 1.100 0.440 0.017
    % Canopy cover 0.116 0.321 0.717 ‒ 0.531 0.196 0.010
    Translocation distance ‒ 0.554 0.316 0.080 0.047 0.208 0.824
    Release time 0.142 0.313 0.651 0.303 0.202 0.141
    Coef. represents the estimated coefficients of each variable. Std. error represents standard error. % Canopy cover represents the percentage of tree canopy cover for each translocation path. Intercept represents Northern Cardinal, CACH represents Carolina Chickadee, and TUTI represents Tufted Titmouse
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Canopy cover positively influenced homing speed but not homing success. This suggests that a slower and more tortuous return necessitated by lower canopy cover (Bélisle et al. 2001) does not lessen the commitment to return. However, increased tree canopy cover does enhance the probability of crossing urban area for forest birds, which implied that higher canopy cover provides safer habitat for bird's movement (Shimazaki et al. 2017). Less forested landscapes present abundant high contrast boundaries and gaps containing 'hostile' matrix for forest animals (Lima 1993; Sieving et al 1996, 2004; Desrochers and Hannon 1997) where predation risk is higher (Cornelius et al. 2017). Birds may choose to move through habitat corridors, rather than across open gaps in the forest, even when a path through corridors significantly increases the distance of travel (St. Clair et al. 1998; Castellón and Sieving 2006). With increased distance comes, an increase in the cost of movement through increased risks from predation and energy expenditure (Baker and Rao 2004). Our findings are consistent with the existence of a trade-off between detouring through corridors and crossing gaps, but our study design did not allow detailed description of behaviors actuating this trade-off. In this study we tested for forest cover effects within the potential return areas, and we could not test for effects of finer-scale configuration (e.g. corridor width or tortuosity; Bélisle et al. 2001; Gillies et al. 2011) of paths used by the birds. However, studies testing specific aspects of actual return paths in relation to homing success could yield very specific and useful prescriptions for configuring 'street tree' plantings within urban areas to lessen resistance to urban bird movements (Shimazaki et al. 2017; Wood and Esaian 2020).

    We reported significant effects of species identity on homing success and speed. Among the species we sampled, titmice and chickadees returned more frequently compared to cardinals and they also return faster than cardinals. All three species are thought to be highly sedentary as adults and permanent residents on all purpose territories they defend in winter. However, cardinal winter movements in large flocks of cardinals may be more fluid than those of the parids in winter (Halkin and Linville 2020; Ritchison et al. 2020). In the center of the species' collective range, mass movements of both cardinals and parids may occur in response to winter weather patterns. Perhaps the biggest difference between cardinals and parids in winter mobility and philopatry derives from their microhabitat affiliations. Near-ground dwelling birds (e.g. cardinals) are more sensitive to fragmentation due to their ground-dwelling habits compared with titmice and chickadees (Sieving et al. 1996; Sieving and Karr 1997; Stratford and Stouffer 1999). Cardinals in our study concentrate their activities near-ground and the homing success was not greater than 50%. The translocation paths we used led through complex and fragmented suburban landscapes that likely presented a variety of obstacles (e.g. Walmart parking lot) and alternative resources for cardinals (feeders) resulting in overall lower return success and speed. In a breeding season translocation study of 10 cardinals moved similar distances (~ 1.6 km; Evans et al. 2017) cardinal returns were 80%, but return speeds were comparable to what we detected in similar suburban (heavily treed) habitat but were much longer in more intensely urban landscapes. In breeding season, birds should be more strongly tied to territory, so it is reasonable that return successes were higher in that study than ours.

    We found that when translocated across complex suburban landscapes, common bird species consistently returned to their home territories, indicating that permanent resident forest birds can exhibit strong site fidelity during the non-breeding season. Familiarity with resources and local predators in a bird's home territory is thought to confer sufficient benefit to warrant the costs of non-breeding season homing (Desrochers et al. 1988; Yoder et al. 2004). Non-breeding season conditions can magnify the value of stable or familiar food resources for year-round resident bird species (Berner and Grubb Jr 1985). Despite the fact that non-breeding season conditions in Northern Florida may seem relatively mild by comparison to the northern reaches of the study species' ranges, a productive territory is likely just as valuable for these birds. Especially in urban areas, predation risk is likely to be severe due to significant reductions in tree canopy density (Patterson et al. 2016) and a simultaneous influx of Sharp-shinned Hawks (Accipiter striatus), a common winter visitor that is absent the rest of the year (Roth et al. 2006; Farley et al. 2008; Contreras and Sieving 2011). Moreover, previous work in the study areas suggests that urban Accipiters may be more highly specialized on adult feeder birds (Malone et al. 2017) than rural hunting hawks. Finally, given the extended breeding season of southern birds, including 1‒2 more broods per year than in the north, territory ownership in non-breeding season likely minimizes intra-specific aggression experienced by resident birds during pairing and breeding season initiation, and this should also encourage non-breeding season homing behavior in displaced individuals (Brown 1963). Therefore, the advantages of keeping a stable and familiar non-breeding season territory with abundant resources for feeding and known escape locations for avoiding specialist predators should be significant for resident birds. The provision of reliably well-stocked feeders (food supplement), almost certainly enhanced the value of the territories to our study individuals, helping to overcome any energetic benefits of remaining near the release sites (Roth and Vetter 2008). However, we used feeder-birds specifically so that their motivation to home could be standardized in some way to enhance the comparison of relative propensity to home range. Whether or not feeders altered non-breeding season homing strength in our study would be worth addressing with further research utilizing non-supplemented birds.

    Possible fates of the relatively small proportion of individuals that were not re-sighted in this study likely include the following. (1) Birds may encounter sufficiently high-quality resources on the return path to cause them to forsake returning to the original territory. For example, Northern Cardinals translocated with radio-transmitters in Illinois during winter largely did not return to their capture sites but stayed the winter at feeders they encountered close to their release sites (MP Ward, personal communications). (2) Birds may have been killed by predators on the route to their home territory. An abundance of passerine predators occurs in winter suburban habitats in Florida, from Red-shouldered Hawk (Buteo lineatus) and two Accipiter species to free-ranging domestic cats. (3) Some birds may have returned but remained undetected by us. (4) The status of a species' pair- and family-bonds in the non-breeding season may have influenced birds' motivation to return. For example, Paridae species maintain close year-round pair bonds and share winter territories with mates and extended family groups (Weisman 2007). In addition, we have observed close multi-year associations between marked titmouse and chickadee pairs at feeders, suggesting long-term interspecific flock associations. These strong social bonds that parids maintain could help explain why they returned so rapidly and consistently in our study compared to cardinals. Cardinals on the other hand are not known to maintain year-round pair bonds (Halkin and Linville 2020). (5) We did not attempt to precisely age the study individuals, in part because of the variance in reliability of available methods among our study species (Pyle 1997). Therefore, some translocated individuals could have been hatch-year birds that were either too inexperienced at navigating to get home and/or motivated to use the translocation as an opportunity to initiate natal dispersal. We note that at least two hatch-year titmice known to us did return quickly and successfully to their natal territories, suggesting that hatch-year birds are already good navigators by their first winter, but we have no estimate of the proportion of young birds translocated overall. (6) Sex may also influence homing behavior by various mechanisms (sex-biased dispersal, hormonal variation, etc.) but with the exception of cardinals, none of our species were easily identifiable as to sex. We currently have DNA-based data suggesting that body size is not a precise indicator of Paridae sex in Florida (KE Sieving, unpublished data) therefore we refrained from using that to test for sex effects. (7) Personality-based variation such as exploratory behavior may have accounted for homing speed and propensity variation within (Cornelius et al. 2017) or between species (Huang et al. 2016). (8) Cognitive and perceptual abilities may affect homing success and speed as well. In sum, we cannot estimate the relative importance of these alternatives (1‒8) given our study design, but all are quite possible, and it is likely that interspecific and inter-individual variability in homing behavior was caused by multiple factors. The relative importance of all of the above influential factors, however, will be of interest in future determinations of how behavioral connectivity interacts with landscape change (Crooks and Sanjayan 2006; Koen et al. 2017).

    We found that responses to translocation vary strongly by species, and Tufted Titmice and Carolina Chickadees were more likely to return than cardinals. Among birds returned, titmice also showed much higher homing speed than cardinals, which indicates the higher sensitivity to fragmented landscape from cardinals. Given the significant travel costs in fragmented landscapes, small additions in forest cover can significantly reduce the time needed for critical movements by forest-dwelling birds. Addition of forest cover at the scale of single land-owners in urban-suburban landscapes (Hostetler 2001), where most of our study species are abundant, could significantly improve conditions for non-breeding season home-range movements. Considerations of vegetative cover in urban and suburban landscapes should be made in conservation planning, not only for provision of movement habitat, but for provision of natural foods and functional nesting sites (Jokimäki and Huhta 2000; Castellón and Sieving 2006; López-Flores et al. 2009). Future studies could productively focus on identifying whether there are threshold levels of vegetative cover or connectivity that define hard limits on animal movement (Metzger and Décamps 1997; With and King 1999). Such landscape level measures could be useful for planning boards and developers of large tracts of land seeking sustainable ecological designs (Leitāo and Ahern 2002). Habitat loss has repeatedly been shown to affect breeding movements (Norris and Stutchbury 2001), dispersal of young birds (Baguette and van Dyck 2007), and we have shown that non-breeding season movements of permanent resident birds are similarly affected.

    The online version contains supplementary material available at https://doi.org/10.1186/s40657-021-00260-x.

    Additional file 1: Figure S1. Illustration of the canopy cover measurements.

    Additional file 2. Dataset.

    We thank M. Avery and K. Keacher for support and field site use of the USDA/ APHIS Florida Field Station. We also thank the students in the Sieving lab for reviews of previous versions of this manuscript and P. Huang for field assistance.

    KES designed the study. CAW and KF collected the data. JB performed the data analysis. All authors wrote the manuscript. JB revised the manuscript sustainably. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

    All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this published article (Additional file 2: Dataset).

    Not applicable.

    Not applicable.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

  • Ali S (1962) The Birds of Sikkim. Oxford University Press, Oxford, p 228
    Alström P, Olsson U (1999) The Golden-spectacled Warbler: a complex of sibling species, including a previously undescribed species. Ibis 141:545–568
    Alström P, Ericson PGP, Olsson U, Sundberg P (2006) Phylogeny and classification of the avian superfamily Sylvioidea. Mol Phylogenet Evol 38:381–397
    Alström P, Davidson P, Duckworth JW, Eames JC, Le TT, Nguyen C, Olsson U, Robson C, Timmins R (2010) Description of a new species of Phylloscopus warbler from Vietnam and Laos. Ibis 152:145–168
    Alström P, Olsson U, Lei FM (2013) A review of the recent advances in the systematics of the avian superfamily Sylvioidea. Chin Birds 4:99–131
    Bangs O (1913) A new warbler from western China. Proc Biol Soc Washington 26:93–94
    Cheng T-H (1987) A Synopsis of the Avifauna of China. Parey, Berlin, pp 805–806
    Clement P (2006) Buff-barred Warbler–Phylloscopus pulcher. In: del Hoyo J, Elliott A, Christie DA (ed) Handbook of the Birds of the World, vol. 11: Old World Flycatchers to Old World Warblers. Lynx Edicions, Barcelona, pp 657–658
    Clement M, Posada D, Crandall KA (2000) TCS: a computer program to estimate gene genealogies. Mol Ecol 9:1657–1660
    Clements JF, Schulenberg TS, Iliff MJ, Sullivan BL, Wood CL, Roberson D (2013) The eBird/Clements Checklist of Birds of the World: Version 6.8 []; download on 2013-07-19.
    Dai C, Zhao N, Wang W, Lin C, Gao B, Yang X, Zhang Z, Lei F (2011) Profound climatic effects on two East Asian black-throated tits (Ave: Aegithalidae), revealed by ecological niche models and phylogeographic analysis. PLoS One 6:e29329
    Dickinson EC (2003) The Howard and Moore complete checklist of birds of the world, 3rd edition. Christopher Helm, London
    Dietzen C, Witt H-H, Wink M (2003) The phylogeographic differentiation of the European robin Erithacus rubecula on the Canary Islands revealed by mitochondrial DNA sequence data and morphometrics: evidence for a new robin taxon on Gran Canaria? Avian Sci 3:115–131
    Eck S, Martens J (1995) Towards an ornithology of the Himalayas: Systematics, ecology and vocalizations of Nepal birds. Bonner Zoologische Monographien 38. Zoologisches Forschungsinstitut und Museum Alexander Koenig, Bonn, p 445
    Eck S, Fiebig J, Fielder W, Heynen I, Nicolai B, Töpfer T, Van den Elzen R, Winkler R, Woog F (2011) Measuring Birds–Vögel vermessen. Deutsche OrnithologenGesellschaft, Halle, p 118
    Edwards SV, Arctander P, Wilson AC (1991) Mitochondrial resolution of a deep branch in the genealogical tree for perching birds. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 243:99–107
    Fischer BS, Martens J, Klussmann-Kolb A, Päckert M (2011) Sing…und ich sage Dir, woher Du kommst! Intraspezifische Strukturierung im Gesang von zwei asiatischen Laubsängern (Gattung Phylloscopus). Vogelwarte 49:287–288
    Greenway JC (1933) Birds for Northwest Yunnan. Bull Mus Comp Zool 74:106–178
    Hartert E (1910) Die Vögel der paläarktischen Fauna. Friedländer und Sohn, Berlin, pp 524–525
    Hartert E (1932) Die Vögel der paläarktischen Fauna - Ergänzungsband. Friedländer und Sohn, Berlin, pp 250–251
    Helbig AJ, Seibold I, Martens J, Wink M (1995) Genetic differentiation and phylogenetic relationships of Bonelli's warbler Phylloscopus bonelli and green warbler P. nitidus. J Avian Biol 26:139–153
    Helbig AJ, Martens J, Seibold I, Henning F, Schottler B, Wink M (1996) Phylogeny and species limits in the Palaearctic chiffchaff Phylloscopus collybita complex: mitochondrial genetic differentiation and bioacoustic evidence. Ibis 138:650–666
    Irwin DE, Alström P, Olsson U, Benowitz-Fredericks ZM (2001a) Cryptic species in the genus Phylloscopus (Old World leaf warblers). Ibis 143:233–247
    Irwin DE, Bensch S, Price TD (2001b) Speciation in a ring. Nature 409:333–337
    Irwin DE, Bensch S, Irwin JH, Price TD (2005) Speciation by distance in a ring species. Science 307:414–416
    Johansson US, Alström P, Olsson U, Ericson PGP, Sundberg P, Price TD (2007) Build-up of the Himalayan avifauna through immigration: A biogeographical analysis of the Phylloscopus and Seicercus warblers. Evolution 61:324–333
    Kocher TD, Thomas WK, Meyer A, Edwards SV, Pääbo S, Villablanca FX, Wilson AC (1989) Dynamics of mitochondrial DNA evolution in animals: amplification and sequencing with conserved primers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 86:6196–6200
    Librado P, Rozas J (2009) DnaSP v5: A software for comprehensive analysis of DNA polymorphism data. Bioinformatics 25:1451–1452
    Martens J (1980) Lautäußerungen, verwandtschaftliche Beziehungen und Verbreitungsgeschichte asiatischer Laubsänger (Phylloscopus). Adv Ethol 22:1–71
    Martens J (2010) A preliminary review of the leaf warbler genera Phylloscopus and Seicercus. Systematic notes on Asian birds: 72. Br Orn Club Occas Publs 5:41–116
    Martens J (2013) Vocalizations of Leaf-warblers and Spectacled Warblers (Phylloscopus and Seicercus). Double audio CD, No. SX 419 726. Syrinx Tonstudio, Berlin []
    Martens J, Eck S (1995) Towards an Ornithology of the Himalayas: Systematics, Ecology and Vocalizations of Nepal Birds. Bonn Zool Monogr 38:1–445
    Martens J, Eck S, Päckert M, Sun Y-H (1999) The Golden-spectacled Warbler Seicercus burkii―a species swarm. Zool Abh Mus Tierk Dresden 50:282–327
    Martens J, Tietze DT, Eck S, Veith M (2004) Radiation and species limits in the Asian Pallas's Warbler complex (Phylloscopus proregulus s.l.). J Ornithol 145:206–222
    Martens J, Tietze DT, Sun Y-H (2006) Molecular phylogeny of Parus (Periparus), a Eurasian radiation of tits (Aves: Passeriformes: Paridae). Zool Abh Mus Dresden 55:103–120
    Martens J, Sun Y-H, Päckert M (2008) Intraspecific differentiation of SinoHimalayan bush-dwelling Phylloscopus leaf warblers, with description of two new taxa (P. fuscatus, P. fuligiventer, P. affinis, P. armandii, P. subaffinis). Vert Zool 58:233–265
    Martens J, Tietze DT, Päckert M (2011) Phylogeny, biodiversity, and species limits of passerine birds in the Sino-Himalayan region―a critical review. Orn Monogr 70:64–94
    Olsson U, Alström P, Sundberg P (2004) Non-monophyly of the avian genus Seicercus (Aves: Sylviidae) revealed by mitochondrial DNA. Zool Scr 33:501–510
    Olsson U, Alström P, Ericson PGP, Sundberg P (2005) Non-monophyletic taxa and cryptic species ― evidence from a molecular phylogeny of leaf-warblers (Phylloscopus, Aves). Mol Phylogenet Evol 36:261–276
    Päckert M, Martens J, Sun Y-H, Veith M (2004) The radiation of the Seicercus burkii complex and its congeners (Aves, Sylviidae): molecular genetics and bioacoustics. Org Divers Evol 4:341–364
    Päckert M, Blume C, Sun Y-H, Wei L, Martens J (2009a) Acoustic differentiation reflects mitochondrial lineages in Blyth's Leaf Warbler and White-tailed Leaf Warbler complexes (Aves: Phylloscopus reguloides, Phylloscopus davisoni). Biol J Linn Soc 96:584–600
    Päckert M, Martens M, Sun Y-H, Tietze DT (2009b) Phylogeography and the evolutionary time-scale of passerine radiations in the Sino-Himalayan region (Aves: Passeriformes). In: Hartmann M, Weipert J (ed) Biodiversität und Naturausstattung im Himalaya Ⅲ. Verein der Förderer und Freunde des Naturkundemuseums Erfurt e.V, Erfurt, pp 71–80
    Päckert M, Martens J, Sun Y-H (2010) Phylogeny of long-tailed tits and allies inferred from mitochondrial and nuclear markers (Aves: Passeriformes, Aegithalidae). Mol Phylogenet Evol 55:952–967
    Päckert M, Martens J, Sun Y-H, Severinghaus LL, Nazarenko AA, Ting J, Töpfer T, Tietze DT (2012) Horizontal and elevational phylogeographic patterns of Himalayan and Southeast Asian forest passerines (Aves: Passeriformes). J Biogeogr 39:556–573
    Price TD (2010) The roles of time and ecology in the continental radiation of the Old World leaf warblers (Phylloscopus and Seicercus). Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 365:1749–1762
    Price TD, Zee J, Jamdar K, Jamdar N (2003) Bird species diversity along the Himalaya: a comparison of Himachal Pradesh with Kashmir. J Bombay Nat Hist Soc 100:394–410
    Rheindt FE (2006) Splits galore: the revolution in Asian leaf-warbler systematics. Bird Asia 5:25–39
    Richman AD (1996) Ecological diversification and community structure in the Old World leaf warblers (genus Phylloscopus): a phylogenetic perspective. Evolution 50:2461–2470
    Riley JH (1931) A second collection of birds from the Provinces of Yunnan and Szechwan, China, made for the National Geographic Society by Dr Joseph F. Rock. Proc United States Natl Mus 80:1–91
    Specht R (1996) Avisoft-Sonagraph Pro - sonagraphic analysis under Windows. Bioacoustics 6(4):308
    Tamura K, Dudley J, Nei M, Kumar S (2007) MEGA4: Molecular Evolutionary Genetics Analysis (MEGA) software version 4.0. Mol Biol Evol 24:1596–1599
    Ticehurst CB (1938) A systematic review of the genus Phylloscopus. Trustees of the British Museum, London
    Töpfer T, Haring E, Birkhead TR, Lopes RJ, Liu Severinghaus L, Martens J, Päckert M (2011) A molecular phylogeny of bullfinches Pyrrhula Brisson, 1760 (Aves: Fringillidae). Mol Phylogenet Evol 58:271–282
    Vaurie C (1959) The birds of the Palearctic fauna ― A systematic reference, Order Passeriformes. HF & G Witherby, London, p 283
    von Vietinghoff-Scheel E (1984) Phylloscopus pulcher Blyth. Weißschwanzlaubsänger. In: Dathe H, Neufeldt IA (ed) Atlas der Verbreitung palaearktischer Vögel, Lieferung 12. Akademie-Verlag, Berlin
    Wang W, Dai C, McKay BD, Zhao N, Li S-H, Lei F (2013a) Microsatellites underestimate genetic divergence in the green-backed tit (Parus monticolus). Chin Birds 4:144–154
    Wang W, McKay BD, Dai C, Zhao N, Zhang R, Qu Y, Song G, Li S-H, Liang W, Yang X, Pasquet E, Lei F (2013b) Glacial expansion and diversification of an East Asian montane bird, the green-backed tit (Parus monticolus). J Biogeogr 40:1156–1169
    Weir JT, Schluter D (2008) Calibrating the avian molecular clock. Mol Ecol 17:2321–2328
    Williamson K (1976) Identification for ringers 2: The genus Phylloscopus. British Trust for Ornithology (BTO), Oxford, pp 10–11
    Zhao N, Dai C, Wang W, Zhan R, Qu Y, Song G, Chen K, Yang X, Zou F, Lei F (2012) Pleistocene climate changes shaped the divergence and demography of Asian populations of the great tit Parus major: evidence from phylogeographic analysis and ecological niche models. J Avian Biol 43:297–310
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(1)

    1. Zhijun Huang, Xiaoping Zhou, Wenzhen Fang, et al. Migration and wintering of vulnerable adult Chinese Egrets (Egretta eulophotes) revealed by GPS tracking. Avian Research, 2022, 13: 100055. DOI:10.1016/j.avrs.2022.100055

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(5)  /  Tables(3)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (250) PDF downloads (27) Cited by(1)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return