Yanyan Zhao, Emilio Pagani-Núñez, Yu Liu, Xiaoying Xing, Zhiqiang Zhang, Guangji Pan, Luting Song, Xiang Li, Zhuoya Zhou, Yanqiu Chen, Donglai Li, Yang Liu, Rebecca J. Safran. 2022: The effect of urbanization and exposure to multiple environmental factors on life-history traits and breeding success of Barn Swallows (Hirundo rustica) across China. Avian Research, 13(1): 100048. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2022.100048
Citation: Yanyan Zhao, Emilio Pagani-Núñez, Yu Liu, Xiaoying Xing, Zhiqiang Zhang, Guangji Pan, Luting Song, Xiang Li, Zhuoya Zhou, Yanqiu Chen, Donglai Li, Yang Liu, Rebecca J. Safran. 2022: The effect of urbanization and exposure to multiple environmental factors on life-history traits and breeding success of Barn Swallows (Hirundo rustica) across China. Avian Research, 13(1): 100048. DOI: 10.1016/j.avrs.2022.100048

The effect of urbanization and exposure to multiple environmental factors on life-history traits and breeding success of Barn Swallows (Hirundo rustica) across China

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    E-mail address: emilio.pnunez@xjtlu.edu.cn (E. Pagani-Núñez)

  • 1 These authors contributed equally to the paper.

  • Received Date: 09 Mar 2022
  • Rev Recd Date: 26 Jun 2022
  • Accepted Date: 26 Jun 2022
  • Available Online: 10 Oct 2022
  • Publish Date: 30 Jul 2022
  • In addition to landscape changes, urbanization also brings about changes in environmental factors that can affect wildlife. Despite the common referral in the published literature to multiple environmental factors such as light and noise pollution, there is a gap in knowledge about their combined impact. We developed a multidimensional environmental framework to assess the effect of urbanization and multiple environmental factors (light, noise, and temperature) on life-history traits and breeding success of Barn Swallows (Hirundo rustica) across rural to urban gradients in four locations spanning over 2500 ​km from North to South China. Over a single breeding season, we measured these environmental factors nearby nests and quantified landscape urbanization over a 1 ​km2 radius. We then analysed the relationships between these multiple environmental factors through a principal component analysis and conducted spatially explicit linear-mixed effects models to assess their effect on life-history traits and breeding success. We were particularly interested in understanding whether and how Barn Swallows were able to adapt to such environmental conditions associated with urbanization. The results show that there is significant variation in the exposure to environmental conditions experienced by Barn Swallows breeding across urbanization gradients in China. These changes and their effects are complex due to the behavioural responses ameliorating potential negative effects by selecting nesting sites that minimize exposure to environmental factors. However, significant relationships between landscape urbanization, exposure to environmental factors, and life-history traits such as laying date and clutch size were pervasive. Still, the impact on breeding success was, at least in our sample, negligible, suggesting that Barn Swallows are extremely adaptable to a wide range of environmental features.

  • Nest predation is a major cause of mortality for birds, and hence has strongly shaped their life histories (Ricklefs, 1969; Martin, 2015; Hu et al., 2020). Nest predation appears higher in tropical areas (Stutchbury and Morton, 2001; Brawn et al., 2011), and also in human-disturbed habitats (Chalfoun et al., 2002; Stephens et al., 2004; Vetter et al., 2013; Vincze et al., 2017). It is thus possible that anthropogenic change may lead to new predation threats for nestling birds and an overall threat to avian conservation (Ibáñez-Álamo et al., 2015). However, there is a great degree of variability among different habitats and regions in their dominant nest predators (DeGregorio et al., 2016), and different predators vary in how they detect nests (e.g., through vision, auditory or chemosensory cues) and how accessible nests are to them (Soanes et al., 2015). Therefore, most meta-analyses of nest predation have struggled to find general patterns (Chalfoun et al., 2002; Stephens et al., 2004; Vetter et al., 2013; Vincze et al., 2017).

    The effect of nestling begging on nest predation is a theoretically interesting question because, although it helps parents understand the nutritional needs of their young, begging also provides a way for audio-sensitive nest predators to find nests (Magrath et al., 2010). Several playback experiments have shown that begging can increase exposure to nest predation (Haskell, 1994; McDonald et al., 2009; Haff and Magrath, 2011), and the addition of loudly-begging nestling cuckoos to nests also can increase predation (Ibáñez-Álamo et al., 2012a). Birds have evolved to minimize the attraction of begging calls to predators; for example, ground-nesting birds, which may suffer greater costs of begging (i.e., attracting more nest predators; Haskell, 1994), have higher frequency begging calls, which transmit less distance and hence are more difficult to detect (Haskell, 1999).

    Given the importance of nest predation to birds, and begging to nest predation, a crucial question is how begging activity, and predator response to it, is impacted by human disturbance. General adaptations of bird vocalizations to human disturbance are well-known. For example, birds have been shown to increase the frequency of their vocalizations in noisy conditions (Slabbekoorn and Peet, 2003). Animals may also increase the volume of sounds in noisy conditions, which is called a Lombard effect (Brumm and Zollinger, 2011). Indeed, a study on begging nestlings showed that nestlings increased the volume of their begging in noisy conditions (ambient noise in the field, white noise in laboratory tests; Leonard and Horn, 2005). This research raises the question of whether it is possible that noise could make nestling birds particularly susceptible to predators. As human-made noise is generally at low frequencies (in most cases peaking below 1 ​kHz; Brumm and Zollinger, 2011; although there can be some high frequency components; Nemeth and Brumm, 2010) and begging can be at quite high frequencies (with considerable components above 6 ​kHz; Haskell, 1999), if nestling birds increase their begging volume under noisy conditions, nest predators that orient acoustically might detect more easily the increased volume at high frequencies where it is not masked. This detection would be dependent on high-frequency hearing that could vary among nest predators: large birds have been shown to have less sensitive hearing than passerines at the upper range of avian hearing (between 5 and 8 ​kHz; Klump et al., 1986), but small mammals like rodents routinely communicate in the ultrasonic (> 20 ​kHz; Portfors, 2007).

    We performed a playback experiment to look at the attraction of predators to begging calls, and begging calls mixed with traffic noise, at a study site with a great variety of predators (birds, mammals, and reptiles). We hypothesized that: 1) treatments that had the playback of begging would attract more predation than a control treatment (silence); 2) playback at maximum volume (the highest amplitude of naturally recorded nestlings) would attract more predators than playback at mean volume; 3) playback with traffic noise mixed with begging calls would attract fewer predators, because the begging calls would be partially masked, especially at lower frequencies (< 6 ​kHz; see methods for descriptions of the exact frequencies involved). However, we predicted 4) that when begging calls mixed with noise were played at maximum volume, the increase in volume, especially at high frequencies (> 6 ​kHz) where the calls were unmasked, would attract nest predators that had particular sensitivity to high frequency sounds, such as small mammals.

    The study was conducted in and around the Nonggang National Nature Reserve (22°28′–22°30′ N, 106°56′–106°58′ E; elevation 59–215 ​m a.s.l.), using the nests of the Red-whiskered Bulbul (Pycnonotus jocosus). Nonggang is a tropical limestone site, where researchers have spent a decade investigating the breeding ecology of the avifauna (Jiang et al., 2013). It is located in the southwest part of Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, southern China and has a tropical monsoonal climate (Jiang et al., 2017). The Red-whiskered Bulbul is abundant in buffer areas of the reserve which includes degraded forest, as well as plantations of sugarcane and the Chinese Chestnut (Sterculia nobilis), and its nesting has previously been studied in this area (Jiang et al., 2015). Its breeding season lasts for about five months, from early April to late August, and we worked on this study throughout the breeding season, during three years, 2018–2020. Nest predation of Red-whiskered Bulbuls has been studied at another tropical site of Southwest China (Xishuangbanna, Yunnan), and been shown to involve a range of nest predators, including birds (coucals, magpies, owlets and Accipiter hawks), mammals (tree shrews, squirrels, mice, feral cats), and reptiles (primarily snakes; Li et al., 2019). Primates, such as Macaques (Macaca spp.) which can be major nest predators in Thailand, are very rare in the nesting habitat of Red-whiskered Bulbuls in Nonggang; the other nest predators described from Thailand are generally similar to those of Xishuangbanna (Pierce and Pobprasert, 2013; Chotprasertkoon et al., 2017; Khamcha et al., 2018; Somsiri et al., 2020). In this region, the majority (~2/3–7/8) of nest predation is diurnal (Pierce and Pobprasert, 2013; Khamcha et al., 2018; Li et al., 2019).

    We recorded begging calls at six nests in the buffer zone of Nonggang during May of 2018. We checked nests every two days to document when the eggs hatched and hence the age of the nestlings. When nestlings were five days old and parents were not present at the nest, we positioned the end of a CR: 152B model Cirrus Research sound level meter (SLM) about 1 ​cm close to the beak of the nestlings. The nestlings responded to this stimulus by producing begging calls, as if in response to the parent (for similar behavior; see Moreno-Rueda, 2005). The SLM recorded 3 ​min of amplitude data, using its fast response setting and A weighting, with one data point collected every 1 ​s. The nestling calls' fundamental frequencies were between ~4.5 and 6 ​kHz, with considerable harmonics up to 7.5 ​kHz (Fig. 1).

    Figure  1.  A representative waveform, spectrogram, and frequency spectrum of (A) the begging of Red-whiskered Bulbul nestlings, and (B) this same begging mixed with traffic noise recorded at close range. We mixed traffic noise into the tapes so that at the peak amplitudes of the tape the begging composed about half the amplitude, with the traffic noise contributing the other half. Subsequently the different tapes were standardized to the same playback volume. (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the Web version of this article.)

    At the same time as we recorded the amplitude of the vocalizations using the SLM, we also recorded those vocalizations using a K6 Sennheiser ME 62 omnidirectional microphone, placed 10 ​cm away from the nestlings, embedded in a Telinga parabola, and attached to a Marantz PMD 671 digital recorder. Recordings were made at 44,100 ​kHz sampling rate and in the.wav format. We then repeated this process when nestlings were seven days old. We took care not to overly disturb the nest, and the whole recording process at a nest lasted for only 10 ​min. Our manipulations did not appear to affect the success of the nests adversely, as four of six (66.7%) of the nests in which we worked fledged successfully, higher than the average success of nests in the area (nest success: 9/31 nests, 29.0%; Jiang et al., 2015).

    In analyzing the SLM amplitude measurements, we found the difference between the amplitude when the chicks were begging (averaged across all such seconds), and the amplitude when the chicks were not begging (i.e., background noise), and calculated the amplitude of the begging (Brumm and Zollinger, 2011). We found no significant differences in amplitudes between the 5- and 7-day recordings. Pooling these two types of recordings together, we had 12 measurements of the amplitude of begging, averaging 73.1 ​± ​5.9 ​dB at 1 ​cm (hereafter referred to as "mean amplitude"). The loudest exemplar was 83.9 ​dB at 1 ​cm (hereafter referred to as "maximum amplitude").

    We placed an FHD-480 model RICH video camera close to (~2–3 ​m away) from three nests to document typical parental behavior, over 24 ​h. Based on the data from the video cameras, we found the frequency of parental feeding was approximately 5–6 trips/h, with the earliest feeding about 6:00 a.m., and the latest about 6:00 p.m.

    We selected traffic noise as the type of noise used in this study because it is perhaps the most pervasive kind of anthropogenic terrestrial noise. We recorded traffic noise at close range because at this distance we found the high-frequency components to be considerable, so that they provided masking towards the lower frequencies of the begging of the bulbuls (i.e., around 4.5 ​kHz; Fig. 1). If nestling birds perceive some interference from traffic noise and increase their begging volume, as the experiments of Leonard and Horn (2005) suggest they might, the higher frequency components of their vocalizations (e.g., 6 ​kHz and above) would not be as masked as their lowest frequencies (e.g., 4.5 ​kHz), and this might make them more vulnerable to predators that have sensitive high-frequency hearing.

    We recorded traffic noise 5 ​m from large highways near Nanning, the capital city of Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, using the same recording equipment as above, and with the microphone pointed into the flow of traffic. We made six separate recordings 3 ​km apart on the highway that circles Nanning on one day (8:00 a.m. to 12 p.m., March 20, 2018). Although the amplitude of the recordings was made so that it did not change over time, there were some fluctuations in the mix of frequencies (0–6 ​kHz) on the recordings: the amount of energy above 4.5 ​kHz could be as high as ​~1/10 of the total energy as a car approached, and less than ​~1/50 when the cars were departing or distant.

    There were two kinds of playback treatments to prepare: begging tapes and the same begging sounds mixed with traffic noise. To make the begging tapes, we took the 1 ​min sections of the 3 ​min recordings with the greatest number of begging calls. Using Raven Pro (Version 1.5, Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology) we filtered the tapes (below 0.7 ​kHz), and edited them to remove background noises, while keeping the original repetition rate of the begging calls. To make the mixed tapes, we used Audacity (version 2.4.1, Audacity Team) to mix with any begging recording an equal amplitude of traffic noise, so that at peak amplitudes (i.e., when the chick called) the total amplitude of the mixed tape consisted of half begging and half traffic noise (as judged using the dimensionless units of amplitude (kU) in Raven Pro's waveform display, see Fig. 1). Begging was partially masked by the traffic noise, although the majority of the power of the traffic noise was at lower frequencies. The masking effect had some fluctuation according to the approach and departure of the cars, with an approximately average situation shown in Fig. 1.

    Playback tapes were then made that were 12 ​h long, with a 1-min section of begging calls followed by 10 ​min of silence repeated many times to replicate the patterns that had been recorded by the videocameras. There was a total of five playback treatments: 1) begging played at mean volume (BEG); 2) begging played at maximum volume (BEGMAX); 3) mixed (begging calls ​+ ​traffic noise) played at mean volume (the same volume as treatment 1, BEGNOISE); 4) mixed played at maximum volume (the same volume as treatment 2, BEGNOISEMAX); and 5) the silent control (SILENT). We made six playback exemplars, consisting of the recordings from the six nests, randomly combined, for some treatments, with the six noise recordings; a set of such exemplars was made for the 5-day recordings and a separate set for the 7-day recordings.

    Small speakers (AM1 Plus Abramtek speaker, China, which have an approximately flat frequency response curve between 100 and 10000 ​Hz) were hung 3–10 ​cm from the nest. Volume was controlled by adjusting the volume of the speaker so that it matched the mean amplitude (begging and noise at ​~77 ​dB, with noise at ​~75 ​dB, producing an amplitude of begging at ​~73 ​dB, all assessed at 1 ​cm from the speaker) or the maximum amplitude (begging and noise at ​~88 ​dB, with noise at ​~86 ​dB, producing an amplitude of begging at ​~84 ​dB, again at 1 ​cm from the speaker) of the measurements of begging. Playback was started at 6 a.m. and played until 6 p.m. for seven straight days, with 5-day old nestling recordings used the first four days, and 7-day old nestling recordings used the last three days. On each day a playback exemplar (one of the six) was chosen randomly, as long as it had not been used at that nest before. In the SILENT treatment, the speaker was placed near the nest, but not turned on.

    In the beginning of the project (the 2018 field season), we conducted the experiment at nests that had been used that year. After the nestlings fledged or were predated, we waited one week, and then reused the nest, provided they were not damaged. Wearing plastic gloves, we first cleaned the nests of any excrement or egg shells. We then placed eggs made of plasticine in the nests. These eggs were made to mimic Red-whiskered Bulbul eggs in their size (approximately 16 ​mm ​× ​22 ​mm) and coloring (marks were painted on the plasticine using waterproof paints). In addition to the speaker, we also placed at the nest an infrared camera trap (SG-660v, Shenzhen Siyuan Digital Technology Co., China) to detect predation events. This camera was always positioned slightly higher than the nest so that the eggs were visible, and was kept 0.5–1.0 ​m away horizontally. We acknowledge that this camera trap can be insensitive to certain types of predators like snakes, or rapidly moving predators (Li et al., 2019); indeed, for 32 nests that were predated, we were unable to assign the predator.

    A nest was considered to be predated if at least one egg was damaged or disappeared. Each nest was assigned to one treatment randomly, as long as nests with the same treatment were not within 250 ​m of each other. In 2018, we used 20 such nests. A problem with the natural nests was that they were frequently damaged by rainfall, limiting the number of nests in which we could put the artificial eggs. In order to increase the sample size, we used hand-made artificial nests, constructed out of dried twigs and vines to be of similar size and shape to the bulbuls' nests and painted to resemble them. In 2018, we installed 40 of these artificial nests in locations where we had observed actual nests over the previous two years (in other projects), but where these nests had been damaged. The overall predation rate, and the composition of the nest predators, was similar to that of the natural nests (see results). We therefore used artificial nests in the last two years of the study (2019 and 2020, 100 nests), again placing them at places where real nests had been located that year (the birds did not reuse the same locations year-after-year). As all nests were located where natural nests had been previously, the sites of the nests were selected by the birds. We did not make any efforts of our own to standardize the concealment of nests, which can influence the amount of nest predation and which predators find nests (Chotprasertkoon et al., 2017; Somsiri et al., 2020).

    Predators were identified if they: a) approached within 2 ​m of the nest on the day on which the nest was predated (Haff and Magrath, 2011) and were detected by the camera or directly seen by the observers; or b) there was no such visual evidence, but there were bite marks on the plasticine eggs. In general, bite marks of small mammals showed small incisions from their teeth or a thin layer of the egg gnawed off, whereas the beaks of the large predatory birds made wide, penetrating gouges into the artificial egg (Fig. 2). To understand the repeatability of the distinction between mammal and bird eggs, we conducted blind tests with five Guangxi University students, verbally telling them that some of the bite marks were made by gnawing rodents and others by gouging birds. We found that only 2 of 70 (2.9%) of their identifications were different than ours. If all the eggs disappeared at once without any pictures (three instances), we categorized the predator as a snake, reasoning that only such a predator might be able to remove so many eggs without tripping the infrared camera. If a nest was predated by the same species more than once, we only counted one predation event. However, in the analyses of classes of predators, we did count multiple predation events if the species of predator were different (but such predation events by multiple predators occurred for only two nests).

    Figure  2.  Several of the predators at the nests, as recorded by the infrared cameras, and the damage they did to eggs. Here the interior of the egg is colored blue (using the photo editor, Adobe Photoshop), to make the pattern of bite marks on the artificial eggs clearer. Bite marks of small mammals showed small incisions from their teeth or a thin layer of the egg gnawed off, whereas the beaks of the large birds made wide and deep gouges. The squirrel and mouse in panels (A) and (B) were unidentified to the species level. Panel (C) shows Indochinese Green Magpie (Cissa hypoleuca), panel (D) immature Greater Coucal (Centropus sinensis), and panel (E) Asian Barred Owlet (Glaucidium cuculoides). The speaker is visible in panels (D) and (E). (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the Web version of this article.)

    To understand how the treatments affected nest predation overall, we used the most detailed information available, the number of days the eggs in a nest were exposed and not predated. Because there was a maximum of seven days exposed, we expressed this number as a proportion of the maximum, and specifically, the response variable was a matrix, composed of the number of days the eggs were exposed without predation (D), and the number of days with predation (7-D). We then constructed a generalized linear mixed model (GLM) with a binomial distribution to understand the effect of playback treatment, year and the treatment: year interaction on this matrix, using the R statistical environment (R Core Team, 2021), base code. Likelihood ratio tests demonstrated that year and the interaction were not significant additions to the model and the model was simplified to the single factor of playback. Results from the model were overdispersed, so we re-ran the model with a quasibinomial error structure, followed by Tukey HSD multiple comparisons, using R package "multcomp" (Hothorn et al., 2008). We then tested three planned contrasts corresponding to the first three hypotheses: 1) whether predation was greater for the playback treatments, or the control; 2) whether predation was greater for maximum amplitude or mean amplitude; and 3) whether predation was greater for playback without traffic noise, or with traffic noise.

    For understanding which predator classes (birds, mammals or reptiles) predated nests (hypothesis 4), we used a simpler, frequency table-based approach, as GLM models did not converge. For each of the first three hypotheses we constructed a two-by-two table summarizing the number of nests predated or not predated by the predator class (with three classes being birds, mammals and reptiles), dependent on the characteristic of the playback (control vs treatment, amplitude, inclusion of traffic noise). We tested whether these tables showed evidence of differential response based on the characteristic of the playback with Fisher Exact Tests. We consider p-values < 0.05 significant.

    Predation of nests ranged between 1/2 and 2/3 of all nests across the three years (Table 1). A total of 94 nests were predated. We identified the predator (or predators, as two nests had multiple events with different predators) that predated the nest 62 times; 46 nests had visual detections (mostly photographs, two observations by the observers), and 16 nests had no visual detection, but did have bite marks. Of the predation events caught by the cameras or people, only seven were outside of the playback period (i.e., between 6 p.m.–6 a.m.; we did not collect the bite-mark information in a way that we could be sure of the time of predation) (Table 2).

    Table  1.  Predation rate over three years and in natural and artificial nests. Sample sizes (n) represent the number of samples per treatment.
    Treatment Predation rate (%)
    2018 2019 2020
    natural (n = 4) artificial (n = 8) artificial (n = 8) artificial (n = 12)
    Natural amplitude of begging calls (BEG) 50 50 62.5 41.7
    Maximum amplitude of begging (BEGMAX) 100 75 75.0 83.3
    Traffic mixed with begging calls, natural amplitude (BEGNOISE) 25 50 50.0 83.3
    Traffic mixed with begging calls, maximum amplitude (BEGNOISEMAX) 50 50 62.5 75.0
    SILENT 25 37.5 37.5 50.0
    Total 50 53 57.5 66.7
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV
    Table  2.  The kinds of predators identified in the study, and their relative attraction to the different treatments.
    Treatment Mammals Reptiles Birds
    Photo Bitemark Total Photo Bitemark Total Photo Bitemark Total
    BEG 3 (Squirrels) 3 6 2 (Snakeb, lizarde) 1 3 6 (4 GRCO, 2 ICGM) 1 7
    BEGMAX 4 (Mice)a 4 8 1 (Snakee) 1 2 8 (6 GRCO, 1 ICGMc, 1 ABOW) 0 8
    BEGNOISE 1 (Squirrel) 0 1 0 0 0 9 (7 GRCOd, 1 ICGM) 3 12
    BEGNOISE-MAX 1 (Mouse) 0 1 1 (Snake) 0 1 3 (2 GRCO, 1 ICGM) 3 7
    SILENT 2 (Mice) 0 2 0 1 1 5 (3 GRCO, 1 ICGM, 1 UNID) 0 5
    Totals 11 7 18 4 3 7 31 7 39
    Most of the predators were identified visually (primarily by the cameras, with two identifications by the observers). Bite marks were identified based on the following properties of the predated eggs: eggs with small incisions or gnawing were categorized as attacked by a mammal, those with deep gouges as attacked by a bird, and multiple eggs that disappeared without triggering the infrared camera were categorized as attacked by a reptile (see text for further detail). This list includes two nests that were attacked by multiple predators. Abbreviations for the birds: GRCO = Greater Coucal (Centropus sinensis); ICBM = Indochinese Green Magpie (Cissa hypoleuca); ABOW = Asian Barred Owlet (Glaucidium cuculoides). For reptiles, we identified one Asian Green Pit Viper (Protobothrops mucrosquamatus), one Chinese Green Tree Viper (Trimeresurus stejnegeri), and one Eastern Garden Lizard (Calotes versicolor).
    a All four of these mice observations were outside the playback period, from 6 p.m. to 6 a.m..
    b One snake observation was outside the playback period.
    c One magpie observation was outside the playback period.
    d One coucal observation was outside the playback period.
    e These observations were made visually by the observers.
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    In 2018, the predation of the natural, abandoned nests (10/20) was close to the predation of the artificial nests (21/40). Also, the detected predators were similar between natural and artificial nests in that year (Appendix Table S1). Therefore, we used the artificial nests in further work and pooled together the results from natural and artificial nests. Predation in the years of 2019 and 2020 was somewhat higher (63/100) than predation in 2018, but not significantly so (Fisher's Exact Test, two-tailed, P ​= ​0.34).

    The number of days the eggs in nests were exposed without predation was affected by playback treatment (Total deviance ​= ​826.4, deviance associated with treatment ​= ​64.5, p-value [using Chi-Square approach] ​= ​0.0057). Eggs stayed unpredated longest in the silent treatment, and shortest in the BEGMAX treatment (Fig. 3). Multiple comparisons showed only these two treatments to be significantly different from each other (z ​= ​3.26, p ​= ​0.0098; although BEGMAX also tended to have quicker predation than BEGNOISEMAX, p ​= ​0.072). Contrast 1 showed that predation was quicker for playback treatments, compared to the silent control (t ​= ​2.38, p ​= ​0.019). Contrast 2 showed that predation did not differ depending on volume (t ​= ​0.02, p ​= ​0.98). Contrast 3 showed that predation was quicker for treatments that did not have traffic noise (t ​= ​2.21, p ​= ​0.029).

    Figure  3.  The number of days that nests lasted undisturbed (out of a maximum of seven), for the different treatments. Treatments that have the same letter were not significantly different, using a generalized linear model, followed by Tukey HSD multiple comparisons. Sample size was 32 nests for each treatment. The box plot shows the median as the dark line inside the box; the first quartile (Q1) and the third quartile (Q3) represent the bottom and top of the box, respectively, and the whiskers extend 1.5 times the interquartile range (Q3–Q1), or to the maximum or minimum exposure days possible. A datum that is more extreme than the whiskers is indicated as an outlier with a circle.

    Of the nine tests made to understand how different predator classes contributed to predation depending on the characteristics of the playback, only one was significant. Mammals came more to playbacks without traffic noise (14 predation events) than to those with traffic noise (two predation events; two-tailed Fisher's Exact Test, p ​= ​0.0023; if four mammal depredation events at night are removed from this analysis, p ​= ​0.030). In contrast, birds came to the traffic noise treatments as much (19 times) as to the other playback treatments (15 times).

    The results of the first contrast, connected to our first hypothesis, indicated that the playback of begging increased visits of nest predators to nests in our study, relative to silence. Using artificial nests (including the use of artificial eggs and/or an artificial nest structure) has been found to give different results than natural nests in nest predation studies (Zanette, 2002; Thompson Ⅲ and Burhans, 2004), likely because the behavior of adults is lacking, either in attracting predators (Martin et al., 2000), or in warning their nestlings (Platzen and Magrath, 2004). Our use of artificial nests must therefore be considered a limitation of the study. Nonetheless, we emphasize that many of the predators that were attracted to the nests (e.g., coucals, magpies, snakes, squirrels, and mice) are typical nest predators of our region (see data from Thailand; Pierce and Pobprasert, 2013; and Khamcha et al., 2018; and from southern Yunnan, China; Li et al., 2019). That begging attracts nest predators has also been shown in natural nests through playback (Haff and Magrath, 2011), and by the addition of loudly-begging brood parasites (Ibáñez-Álamo et al., 2012a). Most studies of begging and nest predation have, however, involved only one or a few kinds of nest predators (e.g., Haff and Magrath, 2011). Our study thus provides a useful assessment of the role of begging in influencing nest predation where there is a diverse nest predator community. It should be remembered, however, that begging is just one of many cues that nest predators use to detect nests. For example, during the incubation stage, visual predators may find poorly concealed nests, and during the nestling phrase they may eavesdrop on parental activity (Martin et al., 2000), so that nest predation may be similar in the different stages (e.g., Pierce and Pobprasert, 2013); at all times other predators such as snakes may search for nests using other sensory modalities such as olfaction (Soanes et al., 2015).

    We did not find support for out second hypothesis that greater volume would increase nest predator response (the second contrast was nonsignificant), while the third hypothesis, that traffic noise would reduce response, was supported (third contrast was significant). In interpreting these results, we must keep in mind the nature of the traffic noise, collected on a large highway, that we used in the experiment. The experiment was conducted in the buffer zone of a tropical forest in which human noise is common, but large highway noise is not, and therefore some responses could have been stimulated by an unfamiliar noise. In particular, BEGNOISEMAX was quite obviously human-associated noise, whereas BEGNOISE sounded to the human ear like wind. There was less, on average though not significantly, nest predation in the BEGNOISEMAX treatment compared to the BEGNOISE treatment, perhaps suggesting that loud traffic noise was treated by the nest predators as particularly abnormal and repulsive; for example, birds responded 12 times to BEGNOISE, but only 7 times to BEGNOISEMAX. It is possible, though speculative since these differences are not statistically significant, that this effect could explain why volume did not increase response. Also, although we found evidence that traffic noise reduced response, the mechanism that led to this result might be different than anticipated: we thought that traffic noise would degrade the signal in begging calls, but it might also have been simply repulsive (see further discussion of the small mammal result, below). In the future, an experimental design that included a control treatment of traffic noise and no begging would be useful to better understand the effect of this kind of noise.

    Our fourth hypothesis was that the masking of begging by traffic noise would be ineffective for nest predators with high frequency hearing, with the implication that if nestlings increased their begging amplitude in noisy areas (as shown by Leonard and Horn, 2005), this could also increase their vulnerability to these kinds of nest predators. We designed the experiment to try to look for this effect: we used traffic noise at close ranges that includes high frequency (up to about 6 ​Hz) components; there is evidence that large birds, like the nest predators involved in this study, may have poorer hearing sensitivities between 5 and 8 ​kHz than do smaller passerine birds (Klump et al., 1986). However, we must acknowledge that we may have failed to produce the correct environment to really test this hypothesis. The masking effect fluctuated with how close the cars approached, and large birds do have the ability to hear in the range of frequencies included in Red-whiskered Bulbul begging (~4.5–7.5 ​kHz). Overall, the avian nest predators in this study appeared to be able to detect the playback sounds (32 responses in 128 trials with playback, 5 responses in 32 trials of silence). However, it should also be remembered that birds, which are more likely to approach nests from above the ground, also had visual cues of nests, as they would be able to see the eggs in the nests.

    We did find a difference in response to the treatments between predator classes, but not as expected: mammals predated fewer nests in treatments with traffic noise (14 responses without traffic noise, 2 responses with it). While the way in which we conducted this analysis followed our hypotheses in comparing broad classes of nest predators – mammals compared to birds and reptiles – it should be noted that in so doing we pool together two quite different kinds of mammalian nest predators: squirrels, which are diurnal, and mice, which may be primarily nocturnal (i.e., four of our seven photographs of mice were nocturnal with a further one crepuscular, within an hour of sunrise; other regional studies have found mice predation to be nocturnal; Pierce and Pobprasert, 2013; Li et al., 2019). It is possible that the lack of visits of mice to playback of traffic noise at night could be attributed to the animals moving away from the area when hearing the human-associated or unfamiliar noise in the area of the nests during the daytime. Alternatively, however, mice could be finding nests through olfaction, without the visual signal of the eggs, since they would be presumably approaching from the ground. Given that we had roughly equal numbers of mice (seven photographs) and squirrels (four) neither taken as a group themselves would have shown a significant avoidance of traffic noise.

    That mammals in particular might be repulsed by human presence (termed a "scarecrow effect" by Leighton et al., 2010), has been suggested in some studies (Miller and Hobbs, 2000; Ibáñez-Álamo and Soler, 2010) and meta-analyses (Ibáñez-Álamo et al., 2012b). Small mammals are also known to avoid roads (McGregor et al., 2008), and noise in particular may influence their foraging decisions (Bednarz, 2021). Another study completed in the same study area also showed much reduced nest predation in conjunction with human activity (specifically the photography of nests by ecotourists; Tan et al., 2022).

    The possibility that small mammals generally, or squirrel and/or mice more specifically, avoided treatments with noise is reminiscent of a growing body of work on how noise pollution can shift the composition of communities and species interactions. In a study of the effects of noise from industrial machinery, Francis et al. (2009) found that an avian nest predator was adversely affected by the noise, with cascading effects on the populations of birds it predated. This is apparently caused by the nest predator being less common in noisy areas. Further behavioral observations on how different classes of nest predators respond to noise is clearly warranted to understand the full conservation implications of human-produced noise (Francis and Barber, 2013). In particular, it would be useful to study these phenomena in a more urban environment where nest predators like rats or squirrels accustomed to human-dominated habitats are less likely to be repulsed by traffic noise.

    SL, QX, AJ and EG developed the idea and wrote the first draft. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

    This research was approved by the Nonggang National Nature Reserve, by the Guangxi University Animal Ethics Committee (GXU 2018-043), and followed the laws of the country in which it was conducted (People's Republic of China). We followed the Guidelines to the Use of Wild Birds in Research (Fair et al., 2010), particularly in the selection of an abundant species on which to work, and a small number of individuals involved (audio recordings of six nests, video recordings of three nests). In recording the nestlings, we took pains to not disturb them long-term (10 ​min per nest per visit, two visits); videorecorded nests were only visited to install and pick up the equipment. In the playback trials, the predators were rarely attracted to the nest a second time, and did not appear to digest the plasticine eggs; therefore, we believe their fitness was minimally affected.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    We thank Wenzhang Dai, Jiale He, Shuaiquan Qi, Xiaocai Tan, and Xiaolei Zeng for their assistance in the field. We appreciate the insightful comments of two anonymous reviewers that helped improve the manuscript. We are grateful to the villagers in Nonggang, who helped in the search for bird nests and were continually supportive. This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant 31870370) and Guangxi 1000 Young and Middle-Aged College and University Backbone Teachers Cultivation Program (2019.5).

    Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avrs.2022.100044.

  • Ambrosini, R., Saino, N., 2010. Environmental effects at two nested spatial scales on habitat choice and breeding performance of barn swallow. Evol. Ecol. 24, 491-508.
    Ambrosini, R., Ferrari, R.P., Martinelli, R., Romano, M., Saino, N., 2006. Seasonal, meteorological, and microhabitat effects on breeding success and offspring phenotype in the barn swallow, Hirundo rustica. Ecoscience 13, 298-307.
    Badgley, C., Fox, D.L., 2000. Ecological biogeography of North American mammals: species density and ecological structure in relation to environmental gradients. J. Biogeogr. 27, 1437-1467.
    Barber, J.R., Crooks, K.R., Fristrup, K.M., 2010. The costs of chronic noise exposure for terrestrial organisms. Trends Ecol. Evol. 25, 180-189.
    Barton, K., 2022. MuMIn: multi-model inference. R package, version 1.46.0. .
    Both, C., Artemyev, A.V., Blaauw, B., Cowie, R.J., Dekhuijzen, A.J., Eeva, T., et al., 2004. Large-scale geographical variation confirms that climate change causes birds to lay earlier. Proc R. Soc. B-Biol Sci. 271, 1657-1662.
    Brans, K.I., Govaert, L., Engelen, J.M.T., Gianuca, A.T., Souffreau, C., De Meester, L., 2017. Eco-evolutionary dynamics in urbanized landscapes: evolution, species sorting and the change in zooplankton body size along urbanization gradients. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B-Biol Sci. 372, 20160030.
    Caizergues, A.E., Gregoire, A., Charmantier, A., 2018. Urban versus forest ecotypes are not explained by divergent reproductive selection. Proc. R. Soc. B-Biol Sci. 285, 20180261.
    Casasole, G., Raap, T., Costantini, D., AbdElgawad, H., Asard, H., Pinxten, R., et al., 2017. Neither artificial light at night, anthropogenic noise nor distance from roads are associated with oxidative status of nestlings in an urban population of songbirds. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. Integr Physiol. 210, 14-21.
    Chamberlain, D.E., Cannon, A.R., Toms, M.P., Leech, D.I., Hatchwell, B.J., Gaston, K.J., 2009. Avian productivity in urban landscapes: a review and meta-analysis. Ibis 151, 1-18.
    Ciach, M., Fröhlich, A., 2017. Habitat type, food resources, noise and light pollution explain the species composition, abundance and stability of a winter bird assemblage in an urban environment. Urban Ecosyst. 20, 547-559.
    Cirelli, C., Tononi, G., 2008. Is sleep essential? PLoS Biol. 6, e216.
    Clarke, K.R., 1993. Non-parametric multivariate analyses of changes in community structure. Aust. J. Ecol. 18, 117-143.
    Crain, C.M., Kroeker, K., Halpern, B.S., 2008. Interactive and cumulative effects of multiple human stressors in marine systems. Ecol. Lett. 11, 1304-1315.
    Crick, H.Q.P., Dudley, C., Glue, D.E., Thomson, D.L., 1997. UK birds are laying eggs earlier. Nature, 388, 526.
    Da Silva, A., Samplonius, J.M., Schlicht, E., Valcu, M., Kempenaers, B., 2014. Artificial night lighting rather than traffic noise affects the daily timing of dawn and dusk singing in common European songbirds. Behav. Ecol. 25, 1037-1047.
    de Jong, M., van den Eertwegh, L., Beskers, R.E., de Vries, P.P., Spoelstra, K., Visser, M.E., 2018. Timing of avian breeding in an urbanised world. Ardea 106, 31.
    De Laet, J., Summers-Smith, J.D., 2007. The status of the urban House Sparrow Passer domesticus in north-western Europe: A review. J. Ornithol. 148, 275-278.
    de Satgé, J., Strubbe, D., Elst, J., De Laet, J., Adriaensen, F., Matthysen, E., 2019. Urbanisation lowers great tit Parus major breeding success at multiple spatial scales. J. Avian Biol. 50, e02108.
    Dominoni, D.M., Partecke, J., 2015. Does light pollution alter daylength? A test using light loggers on free-ranging European blackbirds (Turdus merula). Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 370, 20140118.
    Dominoni, D., Quetting, M., Partecke, J., 2013. Artificial light at night advances avian reproductive physiology. Proc. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 280, 20123017.
    Dor, R., Safran, R.J., Sheldon, F.H., Winkler, D.W., Lovette, I.J., 2010. Phylogeny of the genus Hirundo and the Barn Swallow subspecies complex. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 56, 409-418.
    Dormann, C.F., Elith, J., Bacher, S., Buchmann, C., Carl, G., Carré, G., et al., 2013. Collinearity: a review of methods to deal with it and a simulation study evaluating their performance. Ecography 36, 27-46.
    Fox, J., Weisberg, S., Adler, D., Bates, D., Baud-Bovy, G., Ellison, S., et al., 2012. Package 'car. '. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna.
    García-Navas, V., Sanz, J.J., 2011. Seasonal decline in provisioning effort and nestling mass of Blue Tits Cyanistes caeruleus: experimental support for the parent quality hypothesis: Seasonal decline in parental care in Blue Tits. Ibis 153, 59-69.
    Gaston, K.J., 2018. Lighting up the nighttime. Science 362, 744-746.
    Grüebler, M.U., Naef-Daenzer, B., 2008. Fitness consequences of pre- and post-fledging timing decisions in a double-brooded passerine. Ecology 89, 2736-2745.
    Grüebler, M.U., Korner-Nievergelt, F., Von Hirschheydt, J., 2010. The reproductive benefits of livestock farming in barn swallows Hirundo rustica: quality of nest site or foraging habitat?: Benefits of livestock farming. J. Appl. Ecol. 47, 1340-1347.
    Guo, F., Bonebrake, T.C., Dingle, C., 2016. Low frequency dove coos vary across noise gradients in an urbanized environment. Behav. Process 129, 86-93.
    Halfwerk, W., Holleman, L.J.M., Lessells, C.K.M., Slabbekoorn, H., 2011. Negative impact of traffic noise on avian reproductive success: Traffic noise and avian reproductive success. J. Appl. Ecol. 48, 210-219.
    Håstad, O., Ödeen, A., 2014. A vision physiological estimation of ultraviolet window marking visibility to birds. PeerJ 2, e621.
    Henseler, J., Ringle, C.M., Sinkovics, R.R., 2009. The use of partial least squares path modeling in international marketing. In: Sinkovics, R.R., Ghauri, P.N. (Eds. ), Advances in International Marketing. Emerald Group Publishing Limited, pp. 277-319. . (Accessed 10 Feb 2022).
    Huchler, K., Schulze, C.H., Gamauf, A., Sumasgutner, P., 2020. Shifting breeding phenology in Eurasian Kestrels Falco tinnunculus: Effects of weather and urbanization. Front. Ecol. Evol. 8, 247. .
    Huet des Aunay, G., Grenna, M., Slabbekoorn, H., Nicolas, P., Nagle, L., Leboucher, G., et al., 2017. Negative impact of urban noise on sexual receptivity and clutch size in female domestic canaries. Ethology 123, 843-853.
    Imhoff, M.L., Zhang, P., Wolfe, R.E., Bounoua, L., 2010. Remote sensing of the urban heat island effect across biomes in the continental USA. Remote Sens. Environ. 114, 504-513.
    Injaian, A.S., Poon, L.Y., Patricelli, G.L., 2018. Effects of experimental anthropogenic noise on avian settlement patterns and reproductive success. Behav. Ecol. 29, 1181-1189.
    Jokimäki, J., Suhonen, J., Vuorisalo, T., Kövér, L., Kaisanlahti-Jokimäki, M. -L., 2017. Urbanization and nest-site selection of the Black-billed Magpie (Pica pica) populations in two Finnish cities: From a persecuted species to an urban exploiter. Lands Urban Plan. 157, 577-585.
    Jones, T., Cresswell, W., 2010. The phenology mismatch hypothesis: are declines of migrant birds linked to uneven global climate change? J. Anim. Ecol. 79, 98-108.
    Kight, C.R., Swaddle, J.P., 2011. How and why environmental noise impacts animals: an integrative, mechanistic review: Environmental noise and animals. Ecol. Lett. 14, 1052-1061.
    Kight, C.R., Saha, M.S., Swaddle, J.P., 2012. Anthropogenic noise is associated with reductions in the productivity of breeding Eastern Bluebirds (Sialia sialis). Ecol. Appl. 22, 1989-1996.
    Kosiński, Z., 2001. Effects of urbanization on nest site selection and nesting success of the Greenfinch Carduelis chloris in Krotoszyn, Poland. Ornis Fenn. 78, 175-183.
    Lê, S., Josse, J., Husson, F., 2008. FactoMineR: An R package for multivariate analysis. J. Stat. Softw. 25, 1-18.
    McKinney, M.L., 2008. Effects of urbanization on species richness: A review of plants and animals. Urban Ecosyst. 11, 161-176.
    Merckx, T., Souffreau, C., Kaiser, A., Baardsen, L.F., Backeljau, T., Bonte, D., et al., 2018. Body-size shifts in aquatic and terrestrial urban communities. Nature 558, 113-116.
    Moeller, A.P., 2010. The fitness benefit of association with humans: elevated success of birds breeding indoors. Behav, Ecol. 21, 913-918.
    Møller, A.P., Rubolini, D., Lehikoinen, E., 2008. Populations of migratory bird species that did not show a phenological response to climate change are declining. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105, 16195-16200.
    Møller, A.P., Díaz, M., Grim, T., Dvorská, A., Flensted-Jensen, E., Ibáñez-Álamo, J., et al., 2015. Effects of urbanization on bird phenology: A continental study of paired urban and rural populations. Clim. Res. 66, 185-199.
    Nebel, S., Mills, A., McCracken, J.D., Taylor, P.D., 2010. Declines of aerial insectivores in North America follow a geographic gradient. Avian Conserv. Ecol. 5, 1.
    Oksanen, J., Kindt, R., Legendre, P., O'Hara, B., Stevens, M.H.H., Oksanen, M.J., et al., 2007. The vegan package. Commun. Ecol. Pack. 10, 719.
    Ouyang, J.Q., de Jong, M., van Grunsven, R.H.A., Matson, K.D., Haussmann, M.F., Meerlo, P., et al., 2017. Restless roosts: Light pollution affects behavior, sleep, and physiology in a free-living songbird. Glob. Change Biol. 23, 4987-4994.
    Pagani-Núñez, E., Uribe, F., Hernández-Gómez, S., Muñoz, G., Senar, J.C., 2014. Habitat structure and prey composition generate contrasting effects on carotenoid-based coloration of great tit Parus major nestlings: determinants of nestling coloration. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 113, 547-555.
    Pagani-Nunez, E., He, C., Li, B., Li, M., He, R., Jiang, A., et al., 2016. The breeding ecology of the barn swallow Hirundo rustica gutturalis in South China. J. Trop. Ecol. 32, 260-263.
    Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., Team, R.C., 2007. Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R Package Version 3, 1-89.
    R Core Team, 2021. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. .
    Raja-aho, S., Eeva, T., Suorsa, P., Valkama, J., Lehikoinen, E., 2017. Juvenile Barn Swallows Hirundo rustica L. from late broods start autumn migration younger, fuel less effectively and show lower return rates than juveniles from early broods. Ibis 159, 892-901.
    Ramirez, F., Coll, M., Navarro, J., Bustamante, J., Green, A.J., 2018. Spatial congruence between multiple stressors in the Mediterranean Sea may reduce its resilience to climate impacts. Sci. Rep. 8, 14871.
    Schlesinger, M.D., Manley, P.N., Holyoak, M., 2008. Distinguishing stressors acting on land bird communities in an urbanizing environment. Ecology 89, 2302-2314.
    Schroeder, J., Nakagawa, S., Cleasby, I.R., Burke, T., 2012. Passerine birds breeding under chronic noise experience reduced fitness. PLoS One 7, e39200.
    Senzaki, M., Barber, J.R., Phillips, J.N., Carter, N.H., Cooper, C.B., Ditmer, M.A., et al., 2020. Sensory pollutants alter bird phenology and fitness across a continent. Nature 587, 605-609.
    Seress, G., Hammer, T., Bókony, V., Vincze, E., Preiszner, B., Pipoly, I., et al., 2018. Impact of urbanization on abundance and phenology of caterpillars and consequences for breeding in an insectivorous bird. Ecol. Appl. 28, 1143-1156.
    Seress, G., Sándor, K., Evans, K.L., Liker, A., 2020. Food availability limits avian reproduction in the city: An experimental study on great tits Parus major. J. Anim. Ecol. 89, 1570-1580.
    Seto, K.C., Fragkias, M., Güneralp, B., Reilly, M.K., 2011. A meta-analysis of global urban land expansion. PLoS One 6, e23777.
    Shannon, G., McKenna, M.F., Angeloni, L.M., Crooks, K.R., Fristrup, K.M., Brown, E., et al., 2016. A synthesis of two decades of research documenting the effects of noise on wildlife: Effects of anthropogenic noise on wildlife. Biol. Rev. 91, 982-1005.
    Sievers, M., Hale, R., Parris, K.M., Swearer, S.E., 2018. Impacts of human-induced environmental change in wetlands on aquatic animals: Animal communities, populations and individuals in human-impacted wetlands. Biol. Rev. 93, 529-554.
    Sol, D., Lapiedra, O., González-Lagos, C., 2013. Behavioural adjustments for a life in the city. Anim. Behav. 85, 1101-1112.
    Sprau, P., Mouchet, A., Dingemanse, N.J., 2017. Multidimensional environmental predictors of variation in avian forest and city life histories. Behav. Ecol. 28, 59-68.
    Stoleson, S.H., Beissinger, S.R., 1999. Egg viability as a constraint on hatching synchrony at high ambient temperatures. J. Anim. Ecol. 68, 951-962.
    Strasser, E.H., Heath, J.A., 2013. Reproductive failure of a human-tolerant species, the American kestrel, is associated with stress and human disturbance. J. Appl. Ecol. 50, 912-919.
    Summers-Smith, J.D., 2003. The decline of the House Sparrow: A review. Br. Birds 96, 439-446.
    Symonds, M.R.E., Moussalli, A., 2011. A brief guide to model selection, multimodel inference and model averaging in behavioural ecology using Akaike's information criterion. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65, 13-21.
    Teglhøj, P.G., 2017. A comparative study of insect abundance and reproductive success of barn swallows Hirundo rustica in two urban habitats. J. Avian Biol. 48, 846-853.
    Tuanmu, M. -N., Jetz, W., 2014. A global 1-km consensus land-cover product for biodiversity and ecosystem modelling: Consensus land cover. Glob. Ecol. Biogeogr. 23, 1031-1045.
    Verhulst, S., Nilsson, J. -Å., 2008. The timing of birds' breeding seasons: a review of experiments that manipulated timing of breeding. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 363, 399-410.
    Visser, M.E., Holleman, L.J.M., Gienapp, P., 2006. Shifts in caterpillar biomass phenology due to climate change and its impact on the breeding biology of an insectivorous bird. Oecologia 147, 164-172.
    Wang, J. -S., Tuanmu, M. -N., Hung, C. -M., 2021. Effects of artificial light at night on the nest-site selection, reproductive success and behavior of a synanthropic bird. Environ. Pollut. 288, 117805.
    Welbers, A.A.M.H., van Dis, N.E., Kolvoort, A.M., Ouyang, J., Visser, M.E., Spoelstra, K., et al., 2017. Artificial light at night reduces daily energy expenditure in breeding Great Tits (Parus major). Front. Ecol. Evol. 5, 55.
    World Health Organization (Ed. ), 1999. The World Health Report 1999: making a difference. WHO, Geneva.
    Xu, Y., Cao, Z., Wang, B., 2020. Effect of urbanization intensity on nest-site selection by Eurasian Magpies (Pica pica). Urban Ecosyst. 23, 1099-1105.
    Zhang, K., Wang, R., Shen, C., Da, L., 2010. Temporal and spatial characteristics of the urban heat island during rapid urbanization in Shanghai, China. Environ. Monit. Assess. 169, 101-112.
    Zhao, Y., Liu, Y., Scordato, E.S.C., Lee, M., Xing, X., Pan, X., et al., 2021. The impact of urbanization on body size of Barn Swallows Hirundo rustica gutturalis. Ecol. Evol. 11, 612-625.
    Zink, R.M., Pavlova, A., Rohwer, S., Drovetski, S.V., 2006. Barn swallows before barns: Population histories and intercontinental colonization. Proc. R. Soc. B-Biol. Sci. 273, 1245-1251.
    Zuur, A.F., Ieno, E.N., Walker, N., Saveliev, A.A., Smith, G.M., 2009. Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. Springer, New York. . (Accessed 1 Feb 2022).

Catalog

    Figures(4)  /  Tables(4)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (242) PDF downloads (18) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return