Jing Zhang, Hang Zhang, Yu Liu, Huw Lloyd, Jianqiang Li, Zhengwang Zhang, Donglai Li. 2021: Saltmarsh vegetation and social environment influence flexible seasonal vigilance strategies for two sympatric migratory curlew species in adjacent coastal habitats. Avian Research, 12(1): 39. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-021-00274-5
Citation: Jing Zhang, Hang Zhang, Yu Liu, Huw Lloyd, Jianqiang Li, Zhengwang Zhang, Donglai Li. 2021: Saltmarsh vegetation and social environment influence flexible seasonal vigilance strategies for two sympatric migratory curlew species in adjacent coastal habitats. Avian Research, 12(1): 39. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-021-00274-5

Saltmarsh vegetation and social environment influence flexible seasonal vigilance strategies for two sympatric migratory curlew species in adjacent coastal habitats

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Donglai Li, lidonglai@lnu.edu.cn

  • Received Date: 30 Jan 2021
  • Accepted Date: 05 Jul 2021
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 17 Jul 2021
  • Background 

    Animals need to adjust their vigilance strategies when foraging between physically contrasting vegetated and non-vegetated habitats. Vegetated habitats may pose a greater risk for some if vegetation characteristics function as a visual obstruction but benefit others if they serve as protective shelter. Variation in group size, presence of similar species, along with variation in environmental conditions and anthropogenic disturbance can also influence vigilance investment.

    Methods 

    In this study, we quantified the vigilance behaviour of two large-bodied, sympatric migratory curlew species—Far Eastern Curlew (Numenius madagascariensis) and Eurasian Curlew (N. arquata)—in vegetated Suaeda salsa saltmarsh and non-vegetated mudflat habitat in Liaohekou National Nature Reserve, China. We used linear mixed models to examine the effects of habitat type, season, tide time, flock size (conspecific and heterospecific), and human disturbance on curlew vigilance investment.

    Results 

    Both species spent a higher percentage of time under visual obstruction in S. salsa habitat compared to mudflat habitat but in response, only Far Eastern Curlew increased their percentage of vigilance time, indicating that visual obstruction in this habitat is only a concern for this species. There was no evidence that S. salsa vegetation served as a form of cryptic background colouration since neither species decreased their vigilance effect in S. salsa habitat in spring compared to the autumn migration season. The effect of curlew social environment (i.e. flock size) was habitat dependent since percentage of vigilance time by curlews in saltmarsh increased with both the number of individual curlews and number of other birds present, but not in mudflat habitat.

    Conclusions 

    We conclude that both migratory curlew species exhibit a flexible vigilance adjustment strategy to cope with the different environmental and social conditions of adjacent and sharply contrasting coastal habitats, and that the trade-off between the risks of foraging and the abundance of prey may be a relatively common phenomenon in these and other shorebird populations.

  • Animals use a high diversity of materials for nest building ranging from branches, twigs, leaves and roots to moss, lichens, seeds, flowers and material of animal origin such as spider silk, feathers, hair and saliva (Collias and Collias 1984; Hansell 2000, 2007).Each type of nest material is handled in a specific way before being incorporated into nests which requires specific behavioral adaptations.How has this immense diversity evolved?When novel material appears, it may fortuitously be incorporated.Because choice of nest material and the way in which it is incorporated is partly innate (Collias and Collias 1984; Hansell 2000, 2007), this implies a genetic component.If not selected against, use of novel nest material will increase in frequency.Indeed, exposure of birds to a number of novel materials only recently produced by humans and subsequently used for nest construction such as paper, plastic and wire has resulted in their incorporation in nests.For example, Magpies (Pica pica) are known to incorporate wires and cloth instead of twigs and roots (Zheng 1962, 1995).Likewise, Chinese Bulbuls (Pycnonotus sinensis) include plastic, paper, cloth, nylon particles and threads of human origin into their nests (Chen et al. 2006; Wang et al. 2009).Such materials are incorporated into nests in proportion to their abundance in the urban environment implying that use reflects availability (Wang et al. 2009).This observation demands the question what limits the innovation and the frequency of use of such novel nest building materials.

    Some nest materials have only been used recently simply because they did not exist just a few decades ago.A case in point is plastic used for covering farm produce that was not common in the environment until the mid 1960s when the first Blackbird (Turdus merula) nests with plastic pieces were recorded in different parts of Europe (Cramp and Perrins 1988; Glutz von Blotzheim and Bauer 1988; Stephan 1999; this study).The use of novel nest materials may be akin to innovations that attract the attention of conspecifics.Indeed use of feathers by Rock Sparrow (Petronia petronia) females benefitted the nest owners in terms of differential parental investment by the partner (Griggio et al. 2015).Such novel nest materials may resemble traditional material like large dry leaves that are commonly incorporated into nests of Blackbirds (Stephan 1999).Birds'nests are in most cases well camouflaged (Collias and Collias 1984; Hansell 2000, 2007), making any use of conspicuous nest material selected against by nest predators that readily locate visually conspicuous nests.However, predation is often spatially heterogeneous allowing for differential emergence of nest building traditions in the local absence of predators.

    The objective of this study was to describe the temporal pattern of emergence and subsequent disappearance of a novel nest building behavior, the use of pieces of plastic in Blackbird nests.The most common nest materials are dried leaves, stalks, roots and earth that account for most nest material.Since plastic is not a limiting resource in human-modified habitats, what limits its use in nests?Here I describe the appearance, increase and eventual demise of plastic use in birds'nests linked to the fitness costs of plastic use.I did that by investigating the link between plastic use and timing of breeding, assuming that this nest building innovation increased in frequency through use by the earliest breeding individuals.Furthermore, I recorded nest predation rates indoors in the absence of most nest predators and outdoors in their presence.Because nest predation is much less common indoors than outdoors (Møller 2010), there should be much weaker selection against plastic use among individuals breeding indoors than outdoors.I investigated these predictions in a long-term study of Blackbirds during 1966‒2014.The Blackbird is a very common passerine throughout its range in the Palearctic, where it breeds in a wide range of habitats with particularly high population densities in urban habitats and other habitats modified by humans (Møller et al. 2014).It produces up to four successful broods per year in my study area.Nests are located in bushes and trees and near humans also on or within buildings.Nests are only rarely located on the ground.Only females build nests and incubate, while both sexes provision the offspring (Snow 1958; Cramp and Simmons 1980; Glutz von Blotzheim and Bauer 1988; Stephan 1999).Incubation lasts 12.6 days and the nestling period 13.6 days (Cramp and Simmons 1980).

    The study was conducted at Kraghede (57°12′N, 10°00′E), Denmark, in an open agricultural habitat with scattered plantations, gardens and hedgerows where Blackbirds are commonly breeding (Møller 1991).In addition, Blackbirds breed commonly inside barns and stables of farms, where they are particularly successful (Møller 2010).The study area comprised 12 km2, but 45 km2 since 1987.

    Møller (1991) recorded 169 nests during 1971‒1974 and 1979‒1982, while Møller (2010) reported a total of 503 nests.Here I report an additional 42 nests recorded during 1966‒1971 and 355 nests during 2008‒2014.In total this amounted to 643 nests with 132 nests indoor of which 7 were depredated, while 511 nests were located outdoors with 196 depredated.

    I searched systematically for Blackbird nests in all suitable habitats (Møller 1991, 2010), and all nests were scored with respect to plastic use with each nest either being scored as having plastic or not.

    All nests with eggs were visited once a week, while nests with nestlings were visited when ca.10 days old and again after the estimated date of fledging.I estimated date of laying of the first egg from incomplete clutches at the first visit, assuming that one egg is laid daily (Snow 1958; Cramp and Simmons 1980; Glutz von Blotzheim and Bauer 1988; Stephan 1999), or from the estimated age of nestlings.Nests containing nestlings at the last visit and an intact empty nest on the following visit were scored as successful, while empty nests that could not have produced fledglings, or were destroyed, were considered as having suffered from predation.

    I used logistic regression to test whether the use of plastic as nest material depended on year and year squared (to test for linear and quadratic temporal trends in use), location (indoors or outdoors) and the interaction between nest location and year and year squared, respectively.The interactions were included to test whether the temporal effects depended on nest location.In a GLM with normally distributed errors and an identity link function I assessed whether laying date differed between nests with and without plastic while including nest location (indoors or outdoors) in the statistical model.In a final logistic regression model with nesting success a binomial variable I tested whether nesting success depended on plastic use, nest location (indoors or outdoors) and their interaction to account for the possibility that plastic use was costly outdoors but not indoors.The correlations between variables were all weak with the strongest correlation being 0.38.Hence there were no problems of multi-collinearity.All analyses were made with JMP (SAS 2012).

    I recorded the first case of a Blackbird with plastic in its nest in 1966 followed by a rapid increase first among nests located outdoors and subsequently in nests located indoors (Fig. 1).Nests with plastic almost disappeared after 20 years with only a few observations having been recorded indoors in recent year (Fig. 1).This result was supported by a logistic regression showing significant differences in linear and quadratic terms of year, nest location (indoors or outdoors), and the interaction between location and year and year squared (Table 1).This model fitted the data (likelihood ratio χ2=26.04, df=61, p=0.79), accounting for 27% of the variance.The highest variance inflation factor was 4, and hence there was no problems of multi-collinearity.

    Figure  1.  Proportion of Blackbird nests with plastic located indoors and outdoors
    Table  1.  Use of plastic in Blackbird nests in relation to nest location (indoors or outdoors), year, year squared and the interactions between year and year squared, respectively, and nest location
    Term χ2 df p Estimate SE
    Intercept 20.72 < 0.0001 -518.36 113.87
    Location 31.22 1 < 0.0001 1.82 0.38
    Year 97.74 1 < 0.0001 0.26 0.06
    Year*year 19.84 1 < 0.0001 0.008 0.002
    Location*year 24.93 1 < 0.0001 0.20 0.006
    Location*year*year 7.78 1 0.0053 0.006 0.002
    The logistic regression model had the likelihood ratio χ2=254.85, df=5, p < 0.0001
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Plastic use was primarily by Blackbirds breeding early in the season with a significant difference between birds with and without plastic (Table 2), suggesting that early breeding birds were responsible for this novel innovation.In contrast, there was no significant difference between indoor and outdoor nests, while the interaction between plastic use and nest location was significant (Table 2), implying that laying date was earlier for nests with plastic among outdoor nests (mean ± SE=40.3 ± 1.4) than indoor nests (42.8 ± 1.7), while for nests without plastic the difference between outdoor and indoor nests was larger (Table 2, the significant location by plastic interaction:no plastic, outdoor:66.7 ± 0.7, no plastic, indoor:59.3 ± 1.6).The highest variance inflation factor was 1.2, and hence there was no problems of multi-collinearity.

    Table  2.  Laying date (1=May 1st) of Blackbirds in relation to plastic use in nests, nest location (indoors or outdoors), and their interaction
    Term χ2 df p Estimate SE
    Intercept 1222.45 < 0.0001 52.282 0.864
    Plastic 138.53 1 < 0.0001 -10.743 0.864
    Location 1.94 1 0.16 -1.206 0.864
    Plastic*location 8.22 1 0.0041 2.485 0.864
    The GLM model had the likelihood ratio χ2=209.91, df=3, p < 0.0001
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Plastic use was related to the risk of predation (likelihood ratio χ2=6.73, df=1, p=0.0002).While 29.1% of 556 nests without plastic were depredated, 49.4% of 87 nests with plastic were depredated.

    Predation, nest location and the interaction between plastic and nest location were all significant predictors of nest success (Table 3).This model fitted the data (likelihood ratio χ2=643.00, df=639, p=0.45).Nests with plastic were less often successful than nests without plastic (Fig. 2; Table 3).Nests indoors were more often successful than nests outdoors (Fig. 2; Table 3).Finally, there was a significant interaction between plastic use and nest location with outdoor nests suffering more from nest predation in the presence of plastic than indoor nests, while predation risk did not differ between indoor nests with or without plastic (Fig. 2; Table 3).The highest variance inflation factor was 1.2, and hence there was no problems of multi-collinearity.

    Table  3.  Success of Blackbird nests in relation to plastic use in nests, nest location (indoors or outdoors), and their interaction
    Term χ2 df p Estimate SE
    Intercept 46.24 < 0.0001 -1.248 0.226
    Plastic 3.12 1 0.08 0.451 0.226
    Location 81.43 1 < 0.0001 -1.493 0.226
    Plastic*location 4.57 1 0.033 0.451 0.226
    The logistic regression model had the likelihood ratio χ2=98.31, df=3, p < 0.0001
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV
    Figure  2.  Proportion of successful Blackbird nests in relation to presence or absence of plastic and whether nests were located indoors or outdoors.Numbers refer to the total number of nests for each category

    Blackbirds displayed a novel innovation by using plastic as a nest material starting in 1966.The frequency of this behavior changed over time differing between nests located indoors and outdoors.The first birds starting this innovation were early breeders.There was a fitness cost to plastic use because nests with plastic were more likely to be depredated than nests without plastic, although this pattern was only prominent in nests located outdoors.These observations have implications for our knowledge of behavioral innovations and the spread of traditions.

    The use of plastic as a nest material can be considered a novel fashion since the frequency of use started in early breeders, which are known to recruit disproportionately many offspring (Snow 1958).Such innovative individuals can be considered'trend-setters'by initiating plastic use and through social learning transmit this behavior to conspecifics.There are few examples of the emergence of innovations and the identity of the individuals responsible for such novel behavior.An exception is the emergence of a tradition of washing sweet potatoes by Japanese Macaques (Macaca fuscata) that allowed removal of sand from food items (Kawamura 1959), where the initial behavior emerged in a specific old female.Another example concerns the opening of milk bottles by birds, in particular Great Tits (Parus major), and the consumption of cream, although in that case the innovator was not known (Fisher and Hinde 1949).

    Although behavioral innovations are common, only a few novel behavioral patterns spread (Kummer and Goodall 1985).The dynamics of innovations will depend on benefits and costs of such novel behavior, but also on stochastic events that may cause the loss of innovations.Although novel behavior has been described previously, there are few examples.The present study suggested that plastic use in nests was a signal of innovation ability because early breeding females showed a higher frequency of plastic use than later birds.This explanation may also apply to feather use in Rock Sparrows, which is linked to sexual selection for female innovation by choosy males (Griggio et al. 2015).Other studies have also emphasized the role of innovation as a component of sexual selection (Miller 2000; Simonton 2003).I have previously shown that nest predation is a strong selective agent for birds breeding in association with humans and human habitation (Møller 2010).Two species that have just started this association such as Song Thrush (Turdus philomelos) and Chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs) have a low frequency of indoor nests, while species that have been associated with humans for millennia such as House Sparrows (Passer domesticus) and Barn Swallows (Hirundo rustica) have a high frequency of breeding in the proximity of humans (Møller 2010).The subsequent dynamics of plastic use showed a peak during the 1970s followed by a decline that differed between indoor and outdoor nests.The present study suggests that plastic may have been retained as a nest material in nests located indoors.The disappearance of this behavior was linked to fitness costs of nest predation.There was little or no evidence of plastic being a limiting resource, and most farms with Blackbirds breeding indoors still have ample supply of plastic that is rarely used as a nest material.The fraction of the Blackbird population that breeds indoors is minute compared to the outdoor population in'natural'habitats.Therefore, the use of plastic first disappeared outdoors and then eventually indoors, as the small fraction that constitutes the indoor population was swamped by Blackbirds from the outdoor population.

    I have shown that a novel behavior (plastic use in nests of Blackbirds) spread rapidly by starting out in early breeders.Fitness costs of this innovation were differentially related to the frequency of outdoor nests apparently because of higher predation risk outdoors due to visually searching nest predators.These findings have implications for our knowledge of the spread of innovations.

    T.Duch provided early encouragement for recording birds'nests and their contents.

    The author declares that he has no competing interests.

  • Akaike H. Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. In: Petran BN, Csáki F, editors. Proceedings of the second international symposiumon on the information theory. Budapest: Akademiai Kiado; 1973. pp 267‒81.
    Ale SB, Brown JS. The contingencies of group size and vigilance. Evol Ecol Res. 2007;9: 1263-76.
    Bai QQ, Chen JZ, Chen ZH, Dong GT, Dong JT, et al. Identification of coastal wetlands of international importance for waterbirds: a review of China Coastal Waterbird Surveys 2005-2013. Avian Res. 2015;6: 12.
    Bamford M, Watkins D, Bancroft W, Tischler G, Wahl J. Migratory shorebirds of the East Asian-Australasian flyway: population estimates and internationally important sites. Canberra: Wetlands International-Oceania; 2008.
    Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S, Christensen RHB, Singmann H, et al. Package lme4: linear mixed-effects models using "Eigen" and S4. 2020. .
    Beauchamp G. Flock size and density influence speed of escape waves in semipalmated sandpipers. Anim Behav. 2012;83: 1125-9.
    Beauchamp G. Antipredator vigilance decreases with food density in staging flocks of Semipalmated Sandpipers (Calidris pusilla). Can J Zool. 2014;92: 785-8.
    Beauchamp G. Animal vigilance: monitoring predators and competitors. Oxford, UK: Academic Press; 2015a.
    Beauchamp G. Visual obstruction and vigilance: a natural experiment. J Avian Biol. 2015b;46: 476-81.
    BirdLife International. Numenius madagascariensis (amended version of 2016 assessment). The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2017a: e. T22693199A118601473. 2017a. . Accessed 25 Dec 2020.
    BirdLife International. Numenius arquata. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2017: e. T22693190A117917038. 2017b. . Accessed 25 Dec 2020.
    Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and multimodel inference. In: A practical information-theoretic approach. 2nd edn. NewYork: Springer; 2002.
    Camp MJ, Rachlow JL, Woods BA, Johnson TR, Shipley LA. When to run and when to hide: the influence of concealment, visibility, and proximity to refugia on perceptions of risk. Ethology. 2012;118: 1010-7.
    Cestari C, da Silva Gonçalves C, de Melo C. Keeping safe and fed: large heterospecific shorebird flocks to decrease intraspecific competition. J Avian Biol. 2020;51: e02316.
    Cuthill IC, Stevens M, Sheppard J, Maddocks T, Párraga CA, Troscianko TS, et al. Disruptive coloration and background pattern matching. Nature. 2005;434: 72-4.
    Dingemanse NJ, Wolf M. Between-individual differences in behavioural plasticity within populations: causes and consequences. Anim Behav. 2013;85: 1031-9.
    Dreiss AN, Antoniazza S, Burri R, Fumagalli L, Sonnay C, Frey C, et al. Local adaptation and matching habitat choice in female barn owls with respect to melanic coloration. J Evol Biol. 2011;25: 103-14.
    Driscoll PV, Ueta M. The migration route and behaviour of Eastern Curlews Numenius madagascariensis. Ibis. 2002;144: E119-30.
    Feng CC, Zhang SD, Liu WL, Zhao TT, Cao YD, Xiang-Yu JG, et al. Food composition of five migratory shorebirds at the Dandong Yalu River coastal wetland in spring migration. J Fudan Univ (Nat Sci). 2019;58: 497-505 (in Chinese).
    Foster WA, Treherne JE. Evidence for the dilution effect in the selfish herd from fish predation on a marine insect. Nature. 1981;293: 466-7.
    Fuller RA, Bearhop S, Metcalfe NB, Piersma T. The effect of group size on vigilance in Ruddy Turnstones Arenaria interpres varies with foraging habitat. Ibis. 2013;155: 246-57.
    Gelman A, Su YS, Yajima M, Hill J, Pittau MG, Kerman J, et al. arm: data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. R package version 1.11-2. 2020. .
    Hao XL, Deng L, He Y. An analysis of dynamic monitoring and landscape pattern change of Suaeda salsa wetlands in Panjin using remote sensing technology. Wetland Sci Manage. 2017;13: 31-6 (in Chinese).
    Hua N, Tan K, Chen Y, Ma ZJ. Key research issues concerning the conservation of migratory shorebirds in the Yellow Sea region. Bird Conserv Int. 2015;25: 38-52.
    Kjernsmo K, Merilaita S. Background choice as an anti-predator strategy: the roles of background matching and visual complexity in the habitat choice of the least killifish. Proc R Soc B. 2012;279: 4192-8.
    Kuang F, Coleman JT, Hassell CJ, Leung K-SK, Maglio G, Ke W, et al. Seasonal and population differences in migration of Whimbrels in the East Asian-Australasian Flyway. Avian Res. 2020;11: 24.
    Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB, Jensen SP. lmerTest: tests in linear mixed effects models. 2020. .
    Lantz SM, Gawlik DE, Cook MI. The effects of water depth and emergent vegetation on foraging success and habitat selection of wading birds in the Everglades. Waterbirds. 2011;34: 439-47.
    Lazarus J, Symonds M. Contrasting effects of protective and obstructive cover on avian vigilance. Anim Behav. 1992;43: 519-21.
    Li C, Zhou L, Xu L, Zhao NN, Beauchamp G. Vigilance and activity time-budget adjustments of wintering hooded cranes, Grus Monacha, in human-dominated foraging habitats. PLoS ONE. 2015;10: e0118928.
    Li DL, Liu Y, Sun X, Lloyd H, Zhu S, Zhang S, et al. Habitat-dependent changes in vigilance behaviour of Red-crowned Crane influenced by wildlife tourism. Sci Rep. 2017;7: 16614.
    Li DL, Zhang J, Liu Y, Lloyd H, Pagani-Núñez E, Zhang ZZ. Differences in dietary specialization, habitat use and susceptibility to human disturbance influence feeding rates and resource partitioning between two migratory Numenius curlew species. Estuar Coast Shelf Sci. 2020a;245: 106990.
    Li DL, Zhang J, Chen LY, Lloyd H, Zhang ZZ. Burrow ambient temperature influences Helice crab activity and availability for migratory Red-crowned cranes Grus japonensis. Ecol Evol. 2020b;10: 11523-34.
    Linssen H, van de Pol M, Allen AM, Jans M, Ens BJ, Krijgsveld KL, et al. Disturbance increases high tide travel distance of a roosting shorebird but only marginally affects daily energy expenditure. Avian Res. 2019;10: 31.
    Lu WZ, Xiao JF, Lei W, Du JQ, Li ZJ, Cong PF, et al. Human activities accelerated the degradation of saline seepweed red beaches by amplifying top-down and bottom-up forces. Ecosphere. 2018;9: e02352.
    Ma ZJ, Cheng YX, Wang JY, Fu XH. The rapid development of birdwatching in mainland China: a new force for bird study and conservation. Bird Conserv Int. 2013;23: 259-69.
    McNamara JM, Houston AI. Evolutionarily stable levels of vigilance as a function of group size. Anim Behav. 1992;43: 641-58.
    Medina I, Delhey K, Peters A, Cain KE, Hall ML, Mulder RA, et al. Habitat structure is linked to the evolution of plumage colour in female, but not male, fairy-wrens. BMC Evol Biol. 2017;17: 35.
    Metcalfe NB. The effects of habitat on the vigilance of shorebirds: is visibility important? Anim Behav. 1984;32: 981-5.
    Murchison CR, Zharikov Y, Nol E. Human activity and habitat characteristics influence shorebird habitat use and behavior at a Vancouver Island migratory stopover site. Environ Manage. 2016;58: 386-98.
    Novčić I. Underwater foraging increases the incidence of head-up position in dunlin (Calidris alpina). Acta Ethol. 2020;23: 115-8.
    Pays O, Jarman PJ. Does sex affect both individual and collective vigilance in social mammalian herbivores: the case of the eastern grey kangaroo? Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2008;62: 757-67.
    Pearce-Higgins JW, Brown DJ, Douglas DJT, Alves JA, Bellio M, Bocher P, et al. A global threats overview for Numeniini populations: synthesising expert knowledge for a group of declining migratory birds. Bird Conserv Int. 2017;27: 6-34.
    Pearse AT, Krapu GL, Brandt DA. Sandhill crane roost selection, human disturbance, and forage resources. J Wildl Manage. 2017;81: 477-86.
    Pfister C, Harrington BA, Lavine M. The impact of human disturbance on shorebirds at a migration staging area. Biol Conserv. 1992;60: 115-26.
    Piersma T. Eastern Curlews Numenius madagascariensis feeding on Macrophthalrnus and other Ocypodid crabs in the Nakdong Estuary, South Korea. Emu. 1985;86: 155-60.
    Piersma T. Using the power of comparison to explain habitat use and migration strategies of shorebirds worldwide. J Ornithol. 2007;148(Suppl 1): S45-59.
    Rieucau G, Martin JGA. Many eyes or many ewes: vigilance tactics in female bighorn sheep Ovis canadensis vary according to reproductive status. Oikos. 2008;117: 501-6.
    Schwemmer P, Enners L, Garthe S. Migration routes of Eurasian Curlews (Numenius arquata) resting in the eastern Wadden Sea based on GPS telemetry. J Ornithol. 2016;157: 901-5.
    Si YL, Xu YJ, Xu F, Li XY, Zhang WY, Wielstra B, et al. Spring migration patterns, habitat use, and stopover site protection status for two declining waterfowl species wintering in China as revealed by satellite tracking. Ecol Evol. 2018;8: 6280-9.
    Stevens M, Searle WTL, Seymour JE, Marshall KLA, Ruxton GD. Motion dazzle and camouflage as distinct anti-predator defenses. BMC Biol. 2011;9: 81.
    Studds CE, Kendall BE, Murray NJ, Wilson HB, Rogers DI, Clemens RS, et al. Rapid population decline in migratory shorebirds relying on Yellow Sea tidal mudflats as stopover sites. Nat Commun. 2017;8: 14895.
    Tian YL, Luo L, Mao D, Wang ZM, Li L, Liang JP. Using Landsat images to quantify different human threats to the Shuangtai Estuary Ramsar site, China. Ocean Coast Manage. 2017;135: 56-64.
    Treves A. Theory and method in studies of vigilance and aggregation. Anim Behav. 2000;60: 711-22.
    Ueta M, Antonov A, Artukhin Y, Parilov M. Migration routes of Eastern Curlews tracked from far east Russia. Emu. 2002;102: 345-8.
    Wang Z, Li ZQ, Beauchamp G, Jiang ZG. Flock size and human disturbance affect vigilance of endangered red-crowned cranes (Grus japonensis). Biol Conserv. 2011;144: 101-5.
    Wang XD, Kuang FL, Tan K, Ma ZJ. Population trends, threats, and conservation recommendations for waterbirds in China. Avian Res. 2018;9: 14.
    Xing YQ, Xing JW. Some taxonomy errors in studies on China Suaeda. Marine Sci. 2019;43: 97-102 (in Chinese).
    Zhang AG, Yuan XT, Yang XL, Shao SL, Li J, Ding DW. Temporal and spatial distributions of intertidal macrobenthos in the sand flats of the Shuangtaizi Estuary, Bohai Sea in China. Acta Ecol Sin. 2016;36: 172-9.
    Zhang J, Bai Y, Huang ZQ, Zhang ZZ, Li DL. Community composition and behavioral differences of migrating shorebirds between two habitats within a Suaeda salsa saltmarsh-mudflat wetland mosaics. Biodivers Sci. 2021;29: 351-60 (in Chinese).
    Zhao MJ, Christie M, Coleman J, Hassell C, Gosbell K, Lisovski S, et al. Time versus energy minimization migration strategy varies with body size and season in long-distance migratory shorebirds. Mov Ecol. 2017;5: 23.
    Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Elphick CS. A protocol for data exploration to avoid common statistical problems. Meth Ecol Evol. 2010;1: 3-14.
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(3)

    1. Flavio Monti, Francesco Ferretti, Niccolò Fattorini, et al. Intrinsic and extrinsic factors modulating vigilance and foraging in two gregarious foragers. Behavioral Ecology, 2024, 35(1) DOI:10.1093/beheco/arad114
    2. Fan Shaojun, Zhou Lizhi, Yu Chao. Community structure and behavioral characteristics of wintering dabbling ducks (<i>Anas</i>) at Lake Shengjin, the middle and lower reaches of the Yangtze River floodplain. Journal of Lake Sciences, 2022, 34(5): 1596. DOI:10.18307/2022.0514
    3. Claudia Mettke-Hofmann. Morph Composition Matters in the Gouldian Finch (Chloebia gouldiae): Involvement of Red-Headed Birds Increases Vigilance. Birds, 2021, 2(4): 404. DOI:10.3390/birds2040030

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(4)  /  Tables(3)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (715) PDF downloads (7) Cited by(3)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return