Jianping Liu, Canchao Yang, Jiangping Yu, Haitao Wang, Wei Liang. 2019: Egg recognition in Cinereous Tits (Parus cinereus): eggshell spots matter. Avian Research, 10(1): 37. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-019-0178-1
Citation: Jianping Liu, Canchao Yang, Jiangping Yu, Haitao Wang, Wei Liang. 2019: Egg recognition in Cinereous Tits (Parus cinereus): eggshell spots matter. Avian Research, 10(1): 37. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-019-0178-1

Egg recognition in Cinereous Tits (Parus cinereus): eggshell spots matter

Funds: 

the National Natural Science Foundation of China 31772453

the National Natural Science Foundation of China 31970427

the National Natural Science Foundation of China 31672303

the National Natural Science Foundation of China 31770419

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Wei Liang, liangwei@hainnu.edu.cn

  • Received Date: 24 Jul 2019
  • Accepted Date: 16 Sep 2019
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 03 Oct 2019
  • Background 

    Brood parasitic birds such as cuckoos (Cuculus spp.) can reduce their host's reproductive success. Such selection pressure on the hosts has driven the evolution of defense behaviors such as egg rejection against cuckoo parasitism. Studies have shown that Cinereous Tits (Parus cinereus) in China have a good ability for recognizing foreign eggs. However,it is unclear whether egg spots play a role in egg recognition. The aims of our study were to investigate the egg recognition ability of two Cinereous Tit populations in China and to explore the role of spots in egg recognition.

    Methods 

    To test the effect of eggshell spots on egg recognition,pure white eggs of the White-rumped Munia (Lonchura striata) and eggs of White-rumped Munia painted with red brown spots were used to simulate experimental parasitism.

    Results 

    Egg experiments showed that Cinereous Tits rejected 51.5% of pure white eggs of the White-rumped Munia,but only 14.3% of spotted eggs of the White-rumped Munia. There was a significant difference in egg recognition and rejection rate between the two egg types.

    Conclusions 

    We conclude that eggshell spots on Cinereous Tit eggs had a signaling function and may be essential to tits for recognizing and rejecting parasitic eggs.

  • Sexual dichromatism is defined as the coloration of males and females being different in the same species (Dale et al., 2015), which is often generated by structural, carotenoid- and melanin-based coloration (Shawkey and Hill, 2005; Hubbard et al., 2010). Previous studies showed that bird species can perceive a greater diversity of color than humans (Goldsmith, 1994), such as ultraviolet (UV), and then the plumage colors of birds should be different between humans and bird species. With the development of avian visual models and reflectance spectrophotometry, it is easier to distinguish the sexual dichromatism in bird species which are monochromatic to humans (Håstad and Ödeen, 2008; Dale et al., 2015). By using this method, Burns and Shultz (2012) found that 152 of 163 monochromatic species by human's visual systems are dichromatic for the avian visual system.

    Plumage color plays an important role in sexual selection in birds (Hill and McGraw, 2006), these conspicuous colorations reflecting the quality of individuals, which may enhance mating opportunities or mating success (Andersson, 1994; Dale et al., 2015). Assortative mating is one of the most commonly mating patterns in animals (Macdougall and Montgomerie, 2003). It could occur when males and females with preferred certain traits choose each other (Kraaijeveld et al., 2007), such as body size, body condition, social status and plumage color traits (review in Jiang et al., 2013), and then leaving less preferred individuals to mate with others (Jiang et al., 2013). For example, the Northern Cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis) mate assortatively by red plumage and bill color (Jawor et al., 2003). However, most studies of plumage color traits have focused on species with obvious sexual dichromatism from human visual system, with less attention being shown to species with cryptic sexual dichromatism (Clutton-Brock, 2009; Burns and Shultz, 2012). Therefore, the roles of plumage color of species with cryptic sexual dichromatism in mate choice are still unclear (Rull et al., 2016).

    In many species, plumage coloration can also be related to age (Dreiss and Roulin, 2010; Edler and Friedl, 2010). Older individuals may have more experience in foraging, allowing them to be in better condition during the moulting period (Doucet and Montgomerie, 2003), and they can have superior plumage color. However, age-related differences in plumage color are not consistent among species. For example, Bitton et al. (2008) found that older male Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) are brighter and more blue than younger individuals. The Eastern Bluebird (Sialia sialis) did not change the hue of plumage color with age (Siefferman and Hill, 2005).

    The Chestnut Thrush (Turdus rubrocanus) is a medium sized passerine with biparental care, and it is widespread and locally common in Southwest China (Zheng, 2017). This bird was considered as sexually monomorphic from human visual system (Zhao, 2001), and both males and females exhibit only melanin-based plumage traits. Here, we used spectrophotometry to measure plumage reflectance in wild Chestnut Thrushes based on avian visual system. Our aims of this study were three-fold: first, we investigated whether the Chestnut Thrush is sexually dichromatic from avian visual system; second, we investigated whether Chestnut Thrush mate assortatively with respect to coloration traits; third, we investigated whether the coloration traits vary with age.

    We conducted the field work at Badu village (2200–2400 ​m above sea level) of Lianhuashan Nature Reserve, Gansu Province, China (34°54′17″–35°01′43″ N, 103°39′59″–103°50′26″ E), from April to August 2015–2016 and 2019–2020. The study site is mainly covered by agricultural land, woodland and bushes. More detailed information can be found in Sun et al. (2003).

    We used mist nets to capture adult Chestnut Thrushes during the breeding period, and each individual was marked by a mental and some color bands. The sex was determined by the presence or absence of a brood patch (only females incubate the eggs in this species) (Lou et al., 2021). Twenty-seven individuals were caught before incubation period whose sex could not be distinguished in the field, then we drew blood from the vein in the wing, and stored them in anhydrous alcohol. Polymerase chain reaction (PCR) was used to amplify part of the chromo-helicase-DNA binding protein (CHD) gene by the primers IntP2/IntP8 in the lab. The identities of mating pairs were confirmed by the video of provisioning behaviors during nestling provisioning.

    We collected plumage reflectance data of the Chestnut Thrush using the spectrometer (AvaSpec-ULS2048L-USB2, Avantes Inc., Netherlands), with a AvaLight-DHc (Combined deuterium-halogen) light source and UV-VIS fibre-optic reflectance probe (FCR-7UV400-2-ME). To standardize measuring distance (3 ​mm) and keep out the ambient light, a black metal block was fixed on the probe. All measurements were made relative to a white WS-2 reference tile (Avantes BV, Eerbeek, Netherlands) and were dark-corrected. A black cloth was used as background to reduce the influence of the reflection curve by other objects (Stoddard and Prum, 2008). The spectrometer was set to an integration time of 625.91 ​ms. The diameter of the measured circular region was 6.5 ​mm, and the distance between probe and plumage was 3 ​mm. Each reading was made at an angle of 90° to the surface of the plumage. All reflectance curves were stored by Avasoft 8.1.4 software. Reflectances of captured Chestnut Thrushes were measured at eight patches: two carotenoid-based (bill and tarsus) and six melanin-based plumage (crown, back, tail, chest, throat and wing). We obtained five points per patch from each bird, and we used the average of these five curves for each individual to analyze the reflectance in the avian visible spectrum (300–700 ​nm).

    We chose three plumage color traits: hue, chroma and brightness for each patch. Hue was calculated as the wavelength of median reflectance between 300 and 700 ​nm (Rowe and Weatherhead, 2011). Chroma was calculated by maximum-minimum reflectance/mean brightness (Andersson et al., 2002). Brightness was the mean relative reflectance over the entire spectral range measured. Low values of brightness represent dark colors.

    We applied the Starling (Sturnus vulgaris) visual system to calculate the full-spectrum (300–700 ​nm) reflectance curves through models of avian vision. We used the 'pavo' package in the R software (Maia et al., 2013; R Core Team, 2015), which used tetrahedral color space models to plot reflectance spectra (Stoddard and Prum, 2008). We also set the models assuming standardized daylight illumination (D65) for open habitat ambient light (Wyszecki and Stiles, 2000), which is similar to the habitat of Chestnut Thrush.

    Each patch was combined in principal component analyses with varimax rotation. We kept the first component extracted with eigenvalues > 1, and most of them explained > 50% of the variation (Table 1). We used the factor scores from PCA (hereafter PC1 score) for each individual in the further analyses. For the crown and tail, high PC1 values correspond to high chroma and brightness. For the throat, high PC1 values correspond to high brightness. For chest and back, high PC1 values correspond to high hue and low hue, respectively. For the wing, high PC1 value correspond to low chroma and brightness. For the bill, high PC1 values correspond to low chroma and high brightness. For the tarsus, PC1 values correspond to high chroma and low brightness.

    Table  1.  Principal component analysis (PCA) of color for each plumage patch.
    Rotated factor loading
    Crown PC1 Throat PC1 Chest PC1 Back PC1 Tail PC1 Wing PC1 Bill PC1 Tarsus PC1
    Chroma 0.640 0.477 0.526 −0.529 0.697 −0.699 −0.648 0.634
    Hue −0.356 −0.557 0.644 −0.641 −0.136 0.119 −0.415 0.430
    Brightness 0.680 0.680 −0.556 0.556 0.704 −0.705 0.648 −0.643
    Eigenvalues 1.300 1.195 1.314 1.285 1.292 1.248 1.368 1.199
    % of variance 56.30% 47.62% 57.57% 55.08% 55.65% 51.95% 62.39% 47.88%
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    One-way ANOVA was used to compare the color variables between males and females. To test whether the plumage color traits of each patch were assortative within pairs, Pearson correlation test and Spearman correlation test were used to analyze the normally distributed data and non-normal distributions, respectively. Paired t-test was used to analyze whether plumage color traits of the same individuals changed between the first time caught and the subsequent year. All statistical analyses were conducted in R (R Core Team, 2015).

    In total, 323 Chestnut Thrushes (154 males and 169 females) were measured from 2014 to 2015 and 2019–2020 in our study. The comparisons of reflectance spectra between males and females for 8 body patches were shown in Fig. 1. There were no significant differences in PC1 score of back, crown, chest, tail and tarsus (Table 2). We found sexual differences in PC1 score of throat, wing and bill (Table 2): males were brighter in throat (higher in PC1 score), less chromatic and darker in wing (higher in PC1 score), and more chromatic and darker in bill (lower in PC1 score).

    Figure  1.  Reflectance spectra (300–700 ​nm) of Chestnut Thrushes in eight plumage color patches. The blue lines and areas represent females, and the pink lines and areas represent males. Each spectrum represents the mean reflectance ​± ​SE.
    Table  2.  Sex differences in color patches of Chestnut Thrushes.
    Color patch Males Females F P
    Back (PC) −0.119 ± 1.341 0.110 ± 1.225 1.780 0.183
    Crown (PC) −0.128 ± 1.366 0.119 ± 1.226 2.935 0.087
    Throat (PC) 0.334 ± 1.864 −0.310 ± 1.488 11.750 < 0.001
    Chest (PC) 0.100 ± 1.381 −0.093 ± 1.246 1.880 0.171
    Wing (PC) 0.231 ± 0.994 −0.215 ± 1.415 10.200 0.002
    Tail (PC) −0.053 ± 1.594 0.049 ± 0.930 0.591 0.443
    Bill (PC) −0.239 ± 1.450 0.222 ± 0.979 11.970 0.001
    Tarsus (PC) −0.049 ± 1.135 0.045 ± 1.256 0.271 0.603
    The values are mean ± SD. The first principal components (PC) from the principal component analysis of each color patch are shown in the table. Significant results are shown in bold.
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    In total we measured color traits of 114 pairs, and Chestnut Thrush pairs were positive assortative mating based on plumage color of throat (r ​= ​0.427, P ​ < ​0.001; Fig. 2A), chest (r ​= ​0.295, P ​= ​0.002; Fig. 2B), crown (r ​= ​0.337, P ​ < ​0.001; Fig. 2C), and wing (r ​= ​0.276, P ​= ​0.003; Fig. 2D), but not in other color traits (Table 3).

    Figure  2.  Assortativemating by plumage color in Chestnut Thrushes. Each black circle represents a pair and the solid line indicates the equal expression. Each color was analyzed as the first principal component (PC) from principal component analyses.
    Table  3.  Correlations of color variables in pairs.
    Color patch r P
    Back (PC) 0.093 0.333
    Crown (PC) 0.337 < 0.001
    Throat (PC) 0.427 < 0.001
    Chest (PC) 0.295 0.002
    Wing (PC) 0.276 0.003
    Tail (PC) 0.055 0.565
    Bill (PC) 0.146 0.127
    Tarsus (PC) 0.174 0.068
    Pearson correlation test or Spearman correlation test was used. Significant results are shown in bold.
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    As to 15 individuals (8 males and 7 females) which were measured in two consecutive years, we found that the second-year individuals had lower PC1 scores of tarsus than the first-year individuals: second-year individuals were less chromatic and brighter in tarsus (Table 4).

    Table  4.  Differences in plumage color of the same individuals in two continuous years.
    Color patch First year Second year t P
    Back (PC) −0.170 ± 1.077 −0.429 ± 0.826 0.926 0.370
    Crown (PC) −0.193 ± 0.676 0.025 ± 0.695 −1.052 0.311
    Throat (PC) 0.214 ± 1.042 0.006 ± 1.311 0.732 0.476
    Chest (PC) 0.118 ± 1.164 0.150 ± 1.234 −0.118 0.908
    Wing (PC) 0.115 ± 0.855 0.065 ± 0.995 0.202 0.843
    Tail (PC) −0.041 ± 0.525 0.261 ± 1.124 −1.235 0.237
    Bill (PC) 0.252 ± 0.556 0.339 ± 0.500 −1.040 0.316
    Tarsus (PC) 0.875 ± 0.694 −0.194 ± 1.445 2.280 0.039
    The values are mean ± SD. The first principal components (PC) from the principal component analysis of each color patch are shown in the table. Significant results are shown in bold.
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    Our study found avian-perceivable sexual dichromatism in Chestnut Thrushes. Chestnut Thrush pairs showed assortative mating by multiple plumage patches (throat, chest, crown and wing), suggesting that these patches may function as sexual signals in both sexes. We also found the color trait of tarsus changed across years, but not in other plumage color traits.

    We found that Chestnut Thrushes were sexually dichromatic from avian visual system: males had brighter throat, darker wing and bill, less chromatic wing and more chromatic bill than females. Similar results were also found in Whiteheads (Mohoua albicilla), i.e., males had brighter head and chest feather than females (Igic et al., 2010). Differences between males and females may be affected by natural selection and sexual selection (Baker and Parker, 1979; Dunn et al., 2015). For example, female plumage color traits were negatively associated with nest predation (Martin and Badyaev, 1996; Ekanayake et al., 2015), and females with less chromatic and brighter plumage color may reduce the nest predation. However, the exact causes of sexual dichromatism in Chestnut Thrushes require further investigations.

    The Chestnut Thrush paired assortatively by multiple plumage traits. Similar results were also found in other species (Jacobs et al., 2015; Rull et al., 2016; Zwaan et al., 2019), for example, American Robins (Turdus migratorius) chose mates based on the brightness of chest (Rowe and Weatherhead, 2011). Assortative mating can result from mutual mate choice (Jiang et al., 2013), and mutual selection is likely to arise in species which have similar reproductive roles in the two sexes (Clutton-Brock, 2009). In the Chestnut Thrush, both parents participate in parental care, such as nest defense and offspring provisioning. Furthermore, the population of Chestnut Thrush is relatively large in our study area and they lack breeding territoriality. In this situation, males and females may encounter other potential mates, resulting in mutual mate choice from plumage color.

    We found the color variable of tarsus changed between two years, and this result may indicate that tarsus varies with age. However, the tarsus is carotenoid-based, which could also be affected by carotenoid level in food resources (George et al., 2017), environmental toxins (Spickler et al., 2020) and social status (Karubian et al., 2011). Therefore, to fully explore this question, experiments with hand raised birds are needed. We failed to find any variations of melanin-based plumage between years, which was not in line with McKinnon and Robertson (2008). One possible reason may explain our results: melanin-based plumage color traits were less sensitive to diet and environmental factors (reviewed in Roulin, 2004; Guindre-Parker and Love, 2014), and they may not differ significantly between the two years' data. Further studies on Chestnut Thrushes would be necessary to test the relationship between melanin-based plumage and age.

    Our results showed the basic information on color traits and revealed the sex-dependent color traits in the Chestnut Thrush. We also found that assortative mating pattern based on multiple plumage color traits existed in cryptically dichromatic species. Further studies should focus on the roles of plumage colorations in signalling, and investigate how they influence reproductive fitness.

    The present study complies with the current laws of China and it was approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the Institute of Zoology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (Permission No. 2013/108). Birds were caught only during days without rain and with low wind speed during the capture. Birds were trapped and released into the wild near the trapping locations. No adults died during the experimental periods.

    YS conceived and designed the study. YL, LC, QZ, YF conducted the experiments in the field. YL and LC analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. APM helped in the manuscript preparation. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    We thank Chunlei Jing, Yumeng Tian and Huan Liu for their assistance in the field. We thank the anonymous reviewers for comments on the manuscript. This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 32070452to YS).

  • Abernathy VE, Troscianko J, Langmore NE. Egg mimicry by the pacific koel: mimicry of one host facilitates exploitation of other hosts with similar egg types. J Avian Biol. 2017;48:1114-24.
    Antonov A, Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. Egg rejection in marsh warblers (Acrocephalus palustris) heavily parasitized by common cuckoos (Cuculus canorus). Auk. 2006;123:419-30.
    Antonov A, Stokke BG, Fossøy F, Ranke PS, Liang W, Yang C, et al. Are cuckoos maximizing egg mimicry by selecting host individuals with better matching egg phenotypes? PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e31704.
    Attard MRG, Medina I, Langmore NE, Sherratt E. Egg shape mimicry in parasitic cuckoos. J Evol Biol. 2017;30:2079-84.
    Avilés JM, Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Roskaft E, Åsmul M, Møller AP. Rapid increase in cuckoo egg matching in a recently parasitized reed warbler population. J Evol Biol. 2006;19:1901-10.
    Brooker MG, Brooker LC. Cuckoo hosts in Australia. Austr Zool Rev. 1989;2:1-67.
    Brooke ML, Davies NB. Egg mimicry by cuckoos Cuculus canorus in relation to discrimination by hosts. Nature. 1988;335:630-2.
    Brooker LC, Brooker MG, Brooker AMH. An alternative population/genetics model for the evolution of egg mimesis and egg crypsis in cuckoos. J Theor Biol. 1990;146:123-43.
    Bulla M, Šálek M, Gosler AG. Eggshell spotting does not predict male incubation but marks thinner areas of a shorebird's shells. Auk. 2012;129:26-35.
    Cassey P, Honza M, Grim T, Hauber ME. The modelling of avian visual perception predicts behavioural rejection responses to foreign egg colours. Biol Lett. 2008;4:515-7.
    Caves EM, Stevens M, Iversen ES, Spottiswoode CN. Hosts of avian brood parasites have evolved egg signatures with elevated information content. Proc R Soc Lond B-Biol Sci. 2015;282:772-5.
    Cuthill IC, Partridge JC, Bennett AT, Church SC, Hart NS, Hunt S. Ultraviolet vision in birds. Adv Study Behav. 2000;29:159-214.
    Davies NB. Cuckoos, cowbirds and other cheats. London: T & AD Poyser; 2000.
    Davies NB. Cuckoo adaptations: trickery and tuning. J Zool. 2011;284:1-14.
    Davies NB. Cuckoo: cheating by nature. London: Bloomsbury; 2015.
    Davies NB, Brooke ML. An experimental study of co-evolution between the cuckoo, Cuculus canorus, and its hosts. I. Host egg recognition. J Anim Ecol. 1989a;58:207-24.
    Davies NB, Brooke ML. An experimental study of co-evolution between the cuckoo, Cuculus canorus, and its hosts. II. Host egg markings, chick discrimination and general discussion. J Anim Ecol. 1989b;58:225-36.
    de la Colina MA, Pompilio L, Hauber ME, Reboreda JC, Mahler B. Different recognition cues reveal the decision rules used for egg rejection by hosts of a variably mimetic avian brood parasite. Anim Cogn. 2012;15:881-9.
    Duval C, Cassey P, Lovell PG, Mikšík I, Reynolds SJ, Spencer KA. Maternal influence on eggshell maculation: implications for cryptic camouflaged eggs. J Ornithol. 2016;157:303-10.
    García-Navas V, Sanz JJ, Merino S, Puente MDL, Lobato E, Cerro SD, et al. Experimental evidence for the role of calcium in eggshell pigmentation pattern and breeding performance in blue tits Cyanistes caeruleus. J Ornithol. 2010;152:71-82.
    Gloag R, Keller LA, Langmore NE. Cryptic cuckoo eggs hide from competing cuckoos. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014;281:20141014.
    Gorchein A, Lim CK, Cassey P. Extraction and analysis of colourful eggshell pigments using HPLC and HPLC/electrospray ionization tandem mass spectrometry. Biomed Chromatogr. 2009;23:602-6.
    Gosler AG, Higham JP, Reynolds SJ. Why are birds' eggs spotted? Ecol Lett. 2005;8:1105-13.
    Hanley D, Cassey P, Doucet SM. Parents, predators, parasites, and the evolution of eggshell colour in open nesting birds. Evol Ecol. 2013;27:593-617.
    Hanley D, López AV, Fiorini VD, Reboreda JC, Grim T, Hauber ME. Variation in multicomponent recognition cues alters egg rejection decisions: a test of the optimal acceptance threshold hypothesis. Philos Trans R Soc B. 2019;374:20180195.
    Hargitai R, Nagy G, Herényi M, Török J. Effects of experimental calcium availability, egg parameters and laying order on great tit Parus major eggshell pigmentation patterns. Ibis. 2013;155:561-70.
    Hargitai R, Heréhyi M, Nagy G, Nyiri Z, Eke Z, Török J. Effects of environmental conditions on the egg mass, yolk antioxidant level, eggshell thickness and eggshell spotting patterns of great tits (Parus major). J Ornithol. 2016;157:995-1006.
    Hauber ME. The book of eggs: A life-size guide to the eggs of six hundred of the world's bird species. Brighton: Ivy Press; 2014.
    Honza M, Šulc M, Jelínek V, Požgayová M, Procházka P. Brood parasites lay eggs matching the appearance of host clutches. Proc Roy Soc B Biol Sci. 2014;281:20132665.
    Igic B, Nunez V, Voss HU, Croston R, Aidala Z, López AV, et al. Using 3D printed eggs to examine the egg-rejection behaviour of wild birds. Peer J. 2015;3:e965.
    Johnsgard PA. The avian brood parasites: deception at the nest. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1997.
    Kennedy GY, Vevers HG. A survey of avian eggshell pigments. Comp Biochem Physiol B Comp Biochem. 1976;55:117-23.
    Kilner RM. The evolution of egg colour and patterning in birds. Biol Rev. 2006;81:383-406.
    Lahti DC, Lahti AR. How precise is egg discrimination in weaverbirds? Anim Behav. 2002;63:1135-42.
    Langmore NE, Hunt S, Kilner RM. Escalation of a coevolutionary arms race through host rejection of brood parasitic young. Nature. 2003;422:157-60.
    Langmore NE, Stevens M, Maurer G, Kilner RM. Are dark cuckoo eggs cryptic in host nests? Anim Behav. 2009;78:461-8.
    Lawes MJ, Kirkman S. Egg recognition and interspecific brood parasitism rates in red bishops (Aves: Ploceidae). Anim Behav. 1996;52:553-63.
    Liang W, Møller AP, Stokke BG, Yang C, Kovařík P, Wang H, et al. Geographic variation in egg ejection rate by great tits across 2 continents. Behav Ecol. 2016;27:1405-12.
    Liu J, Ma L, Zhang Z, Gu D, Wang J, Li J, et al. Maximum frequency of songs reflects body size among male dusky warblers Phylloscopus fuscatus (Passeriformes: Phylloscopidae). Eur Zool J. 2017;84:186-92.
    López-De-Hierro MDG, DeNeve L. Pigment limitation and female reproductive characteristics influence eggshell spottiness and ground colour variation in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus). J Ornithol. 2010;151:833-40.
    López-De-Hierro MDG, Moreno-Rueda G. Egg-spot pattern rather than egg colour affects conspecific egg rejection in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2009;64:317-24.
    Luro AB, Igic B, Croston R, López AV, Shawkey MD, Hauber ME. Which egg features predict egg rejection responses in American robins? Replicating Rothstein's (1982) study. Ecol Evol. 2018;8:1673-9.
    Marchetti K. Egg rejection in a passerine bird: size does matter. Anim Behav. 2000;59:877-83.
    Mason P, Rothstein SI. Coevolution and avian brood parasitism: cowbird eggs show evolutionary response to host discrimination. Evolution. 1986;40:1207-14.
    Maurer G, Portugal SJ, Cassey P. An embryo's eye view of avian eggshell pigmentation. J Avian Biol. 2011;42:494-504.
    Medina I, Troscianko J, Stevens M, Langmore NE. Brood parasitism is linked to egg pattern diversity within and among species of Australian passerines. Am Nat. 2016;187:351-62.
    Meshcheryagina SG, Mashanova A, Bachurin GN, Mitiay IS, Golovatin MG. Host species determines egg size in Oriental cuckoo. J Zool. 2018;306:147-55.
    Mikšík I, Holáň V, Deyl Z. Avian eggshell pigments and their variability. Comp Biochem Physiol B Biochem Mol Biol. 1996;113:607-12.
    Newton AV. A dictionary of birds. London: A & C Black; 1896.
    Päckert M, Martens J, Eck S, Nazarenko AA, Valchuk OP, Petri B, et al. The great tit (Parus major)—a misclassified ring species. Biol J Linn Soc. 2005;86:153-74.
    Payne RB. The cuckoos. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005.
    Poláček M, Griggio M, Mikšík I, Bartíková M, Eckenfellner M, Hoi H. Eggshell coloration and its importance in postmating sexual selection. Ecol Evol. 2017;7:941-9.
    Polačiková L, Grim T. Blunt egg pole holds cues for foreign egg discrimination: experimental evidence. J Avian Biol. 2010;41:111-6.
    Polačiková L, Honza M, Procházka P, Topercer J, Stokke BG. Colour characteristics of the blunt egg pole: cues for recognition of parasitic eggs as revealed by reflectance spectrophotometry. Anim Behav. 2007;74:419-27.
    Polačiková L, Stokke BG, Procházka P, Honza M, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. The role of blunt egg pole characteristics for recognition of eggs in the song thrush (Turdus philomelos). Behaviour. 2010;147:465-78.
    Reynolds SJ, Martin GR, Cassey P. Is sexual selection blurring the functional significance of eggshell colouration hypotheses? Anim Behav. 2009;78:209-15.
    Rothstein SI. Mechanisms of avian egg-recognition—additional evidence for learned components. Anim Behav. 1978;26:671-6.
    Rothstein SI. Mechanisms of avian egg recognition: which egg parameters elicit responses by rejecter species? Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1982;11:229-39.
    Rothstein SI. A model system for coevolution: avian brood parasitism. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1990;21:481-508.
    Sanz JJ, García-Navas V. Eggshell pigmentation pattern in relation to breeding performance of blue tits Cyanistes caeruleus. J Anim Ecol. 2009;78:31-41.
    Soler M. Long-term coevolution between avian brood parasites and their hosts. Biol Rev. 2014;89:688-704.
    Soler JJ, Møller AP. A comparative analysis of the evolution of variation in appearance of eggs of European passerines in relation to brood parasitism. Behav Ecol. 1996;7:89-94.
    Spottiswoode CN, Stevens M. Visual modeling shows that avian host parents use multiple visual cues in rejecting parasitic eggs. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107:8672-6.
    Starling M, Heinsohn R, Cockburn A, Langmore NE. Cryptic gentes revealed in pallid cuckoos Cuculus pallidus using reflectance spectrophotometry. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2006;273:1929-34.
    Stoddard MC, Stevens M. Pattern mimicry of host eggs by the common cuckoo, as seen through a bird's eye. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2010;277:1387-93.
    Stoddard MC, Stevens M. Avian vision and the evolution of egg color mimicry in the common cuckoo. Evolution. 2011;65:2004-13.
    Stoddard MC, Fayet A, Kilner RM, Hinde C. Egg spots patterns do not advertise offspring quality or influence male provisioning in great tits. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e40211.
    Stoddard MC, Kilner RM, Town C. Pattern recognition algorithm reveals how birds evolve individual egg pattern signatures. Nat Commun. 2014;5:4117.
    Stokke BG, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. Obligate brood parasites as selective agents for evolution of egg appearance in passerine birds. Evolution. 2002;56:199-205.
    Stokke BG, Takasu F, Moksnes A, Røskaft E. The importance of clutch characteristics and learning for anti-parasite adaptations in hosts of avian brood parasites. Evolution. 2007;61:2212-28.
    Stokke BG, Røskaft E, Moksnes A, Møller AP, Antonov A, Fossøy F, et al. Disappearance of eggs from nonparasitized nests of brood parasite hosts: the evolutionary equilibrium hypothesis revisited. Biol J Linn Soc. 2016;118:215-25.
    Stokke BG, Fossøy F, Røskaft E, Moksnes A. Adaptations of brood parasitic eggs. In: Soler M, editor. Avian brood parasitism: behaviour, ecology, evolution and coevolution. Cham: Springer International Publishing AG; 2017. p. 363-84.
    Šulc M, Procházka P, Capek M, Honza M. Birds use eggshell UV reflectance when recognizing non-mimetic parasitic eggs. Behav Ecol. 2016;27:677-84.
    Šulc M, Troscianko J, Štětková G, Hughes AE, Jelínek V, Capek M, Honza M. Mimicry cannot explain rejection type in a host-brood parasite system. Anim Behav. 2019;155:111-8.
    Swynnerton CFM. Rejections by birds of eggs unlike their own: with remarks on some of the cuckoo problems. Ibis. 1918;6:127-54.
    Takasu F. Co-evolutionary dynamics of egg appearance in avian brood parasitism. Evol Ecol Res. 2003;5:345-62.
    Underwood TJ, Sealy SG. Parameters of brown headed cowbird Molothrus ater egg discrimination in warbling vireos Vireo gilvus. J Avian Biol. 2006;37:457-66.
    Yang C, Liang W, Cai Y, Shi S, Takasu F, Møller AP, et al. Coevolution in action: disruptive selection on egg colour in an avian brood parasite and its host. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e10816.
    Yang C, Liang W, Møller AP. Egg retrieval versus egg rejection in cuckoo hosts. Philos Trans R Soc B. 2019;374:20180200.
    Yu J, Lv W, Xu H, Bibi N, Yu Y, Jiang Y, et al. Function of note strings in Japanese Tit alarm calls to the common cuckoo: a playback experiment. Avian Res. 2017;8:22.
  • Related Articles

Catalog

    Figures(2)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (563) PDF downloads (8) Cited by()

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return