Ines G. Moran, Katrina C. Lukianchuk, Stéphanie. M. Doucet, Amy E. M. Newma, Heather Williams, D. Ryan Norris, Daniel J. Mennill. 2019: Diel and seasonal patterns of variation in the singing behaviour of Savannah Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis). Avian Research, 10(1): 26. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-019-0165-6
Citation: Ines G. Moran, Katrina C. Lukianchuk, Stéphanie. M. Doucet, Amy E. M. Newma, Heather Williams, D. Ryan Norris, Daniel J. Mennill. 2019: Diel and seasonal patterns of variation in the singing behaviour of Savannah Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis). Avian Research, 10(1): 26. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-019-0165-6

Diel and seasonal patterns of variation in the singing behaviour of Savannah Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis)

Funds: 

We thank the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada NSERC for ongoing financial support to SMD, AEM, DRN, and DJM 

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Ines G. Moran, moran11@uwindsor.ca

    Daniel J. Mennill, dmennill@uwindsor.ca

  • Received Date: 14 Feb 2019
  • Accepted Date: 27 Jun 2019
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 30 Jun 2019
  • Background 

    The vocalizations of birds are dynamic traits that often vary in output with time of day and time of year. By quantifying patterns of diel and seasonal variation in vocal output, we can gain insight into the ecology and evolution of birds and the function of their vocalizations. In this investigation, we quantified diel and seasonal variation in song output by studying a breeding population of Savannah Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis).

    Methods 

    We used autonomous recorders to collect extensive recordings across the breeding season in a long-term, colour-marked study population of Savannah Sparrows in eastern Canada. We described diel and seasonal variation across five different breeding stages based on recordings of more than 50, 000 songs from 34 males.

    Results 

    During the pre-breeding stage, prior to female arrival, males sang a pronounced dawn chorus with a peak in song output during the early morning. During the breeding stage, in contrast, the dawn chorus was diminished and males instead exhibited a pronounced dusk chorus with a peak in song output during the evening. Across the breeding season, the highest levels of song output occurred in late April and early May, soon after males arrived on the breeding grounds, and the lowest levels occurred in August, prior to the departure of birds for the wintering grounds.

    Conclusions 

    These patterns suggest that Savannah Sparrows' early-season dawn choruses are directed at males and serve a territorial defense function. Later-season dusk choruses, in contrast, appear to play a role in attracting mates and possibly acoustic mate guarding.

  • The most dominant global threat to natural forests and their biodiversity is land-cover change, which has negative impacts on both species persistence and ecosystem functions (Fahrig 2003; Tylianakis et al. 2008; Pogson 2015; Holyoak and Heath 2016). In Asia, many natural forests have been transformed to farmlands by local residents, resulting in several natural patches of forest within highly human-disturbed habitats (Foley et al. 2005; Haddad et al. 2015). Therefore, it is important to understand the effects of land-cover change on plant regeneration in these disturbed habitats (Cordeiro and Howe 2003; Markl et al. 2012; Bai et al. 2017).

    In general, seed-dispersal interactions between animals and fleshy-fruited plants form the basis of plant regeneration, which is one of the key factors influencing the persistence of plant communities (Janzen 1970). In various disturbed habitats, the seed dispersal pattern of frugivorous birds directly reflects their functional traits, especially their morphological traits (Larsen et al. 2005; Suding et al. 2008; Schupp et al. 2010; Bregman et al. 2016; Farwig et al. 2017). As many ecologists predicted, the body size of birds can be used to estimate the ecological role of birds in seed removal and can affect the degree of species-specific interactions in seed-dispersal distance (Perez-Méndez et al. 2015; Bregman et al. 2016; Muñoz et al. 2017). Large-bodied birds can transport more seeds than small birds, and have the potential to facilitate seed dispersal among forest fragments (patches) owing to their longer dispersal distance (Spiegel and Nathan 2007; Wotton and Kelly 2012; Mueller et al. 2014; Li et al. 2017). However, increasing scientific evidence supports the concept that seed dispersal could be promoted by several bird functional traits (Schleuning et al. 2015; Muñoz et al. 2017). Thus, enhanced seed dispersal can be affected by not only bird morphological traits (e.g. body length, weight, wing length, tail length, etc.), but also by bird behavioural traits (e.g. foraging behaviour, flight behaviour, etc.) (Bregman et al. 2016; Farwig et al. 2017). Owing to the inherent difficulty of monitoring multiple bird functional traits in different disturbed habitats, empirical evidence supporting the extent of the role of bird functional traits in seed dispersal mutualism remains limited.

    In the present study, we assessed the contributions of bird functional traits (behavioural traits: food habit, foraging pattern, foraging frequency, and habitat specialisation; morphological traits: weight, body length, wing length, and tail length) to both seed removal patterns and seed dispersal distance of an endangered and native tree species, Chinese yew (Taxus chinensis (Rehder and E.H. Wilson) Rehder), across farmland, patchy habitat, and natural habitat. We hypothesised that the seed removal patterns of these endangered trees vary across disturbed habitats and are strongly affected by bird foraging (behavioural) functional traits. In addition, we also tested how the bird flight (morphological) functional traits influence the seed dispersal distance across disturbed habitats.

    Taxus chinensis is a dioecious, wind-pollinated species, distributed in evergreen broadleaf forests in China. Each year, female plants bear axillary cones, which develop into fleshy arils (commonly referred to as "fruits"), each containing a single seed, in autumn; an average Chinese yew tree bears more than 4000 fruits annually (Li et al. 2015, 2017).

    We selected three populations of T. chinensis in the villages of Chongtou and Guihe, which are located in the southern experimental area of the Meihua Mountain National Nature Reserve (MMNNR), in southwest Fujian Province, China (25°15′-25°35′N, 116°45′-116°57′E). The composition of the three populations are as follows: (1) a farmland population from Guihe that consists of two remnant mother trees in Capsicum annuum farmland (northeast slope; slope angle 15°; altitude 1180 m; tree coverage 5%); (2) a natural population from Guihe that consists of ten mother trees scattered in the evergreen broadleaf forest (northwest slope; slope angle 32°; altitude 1178 m; tree coverage 80%); and (3) a patchy population, located in Chongtou, that contains the largest wild T. chinensis population in China (approximately 490 adults). This patch of T. chinensis is interlaced with patches of farmland, bamboo, mixed bamboo, and broadleaf trees to form a fragmented landscape (northeast slope; slope angle 27°; altitude 1218 m; tree coverage 70%) (Li et al. 2015).

    To quantify how birds visited fruiting trees and how many seeds were removed from T. chinensis plants across the different sites, field observations were performed during the fruiting season (late October to early December) in all three habitats (farmland and natural habitat: 2012 and 2013; patchy habitat: 2011 and 2012). Observation points were located on opposing mountain slopes, and observations were made using a field scope (Leica 70, Leica Camera AG, Wetzlar, Germany) at distances ranging from 50 to 100 m. Each site was observed from 06:30 to 18:30 on observation days, for a total of 720 h of observation. During the observations, fruit-eating birds were identified and counted, and the foraging pattern (pecking, swallowing) and number of fruits they removed was recorded (i.e., fruits were either ingested on the tree or carried in the beak upon leaving the tree). From these records, we compared the number of seeds removed and frequency of foraging by frugivorous birds across the three habitats (number of fruits per visit × foraging frequencies) (Breitbach et al. 2010).

    To quantify how far T. chinensis seeds were transported by birds, post-foraging movement behaviours of birds were studied in all three habitats (farmland, patchy, and natural). Each potential disperser leaving a given tree was tracked in a session that ended once visual contact was lost or if the focal bird could no longer be distinguished from its conspecifics. For each tracking session, the perching position of each bird was recorded every 30 s after it left the study tree, and observers noted nearby characteristic landmarks for subsequent verification of field distance estimates using a map. To estimate the real seed dispersal distance, we collected seeds that had been regurgitated by birds, and had fallen on the forest floor, the morning after an observation by finding the marked perching positions (regurgitated seeds refer to cleaned seeds without any aril that had been totally digested by birds) (Pan et al. 2016). We also assumed that the position in which the bird perched was a sufficient proxy for evaluating seed dispersal distance, as previously described (Breitbach et al. 2010). From these records, we compared seed dispersal distances across the three habitat types.

    To quantify how bird functional traits affected seed dispersal patterns, we selected eight functional traits that are functionally related to the foraging and post-foraging flight behaviours of frugivorous birds (Moermond and Denslow 1985) as follows: foraging behaviours (food habit, foraging pattern, foraging frequency, weight, and length) and flight performance behaviours (wing length, tail length, weight, length, and habitat specialisation). All morphological traits were measured using two male and two female museum specimens of each bird species from local populations. In addition, data on food habits (frugivorous, omnivorous) and habitat specialisation (generalist, specialist) were compiled from A Field Guide to the Birds of China (MacKinnon et al. 2000). For the analyses, we used the mean measurement of each trait for each species. All morphological traits were log-transformed to approximate normality.

    To quantify how bird functional traits affected seed removal, we first used a machine-learning algorithm (random forest model) to plot the partial effects of food habit, foraging pattern, foraging frequency, weight, body length, site, and year against the number of seeds removed (R package Random Forest) (Breiman 2001). Moreover, we also used the random forest model to plot the partial effects of bird flight traits (wing length, tail length, weight, body length, and habitat specialisation) on seed dispersal distance (R package Random Forest) (Breiman 2001).

    During the fruiting season, 5106 seeds (2012: 2341 seeds; 2013: 2765 seeds), 1091 seeds (2012: 719 seeds; 2013: 372 seeds), and 10, 277 seeds (2011: 4704 seeds; 2012: 5573 seeds) were removed by 15, 13, and 22 bird species in the farmland, natural habitat, and patchy habitat, respectively. More seeds were removed from the patchy habitat than from the other two habitats. The most common forager at the farmland site was the Collared Finchbill (Spizixos semitorques) (2012: 42 foraging events, 714 seeds; 2013: 40 foraging events, 690 seeds), whereas the Orange-bellied Leafbird (Chloropsis hardwickii) (2012: 23 foraging events, 184 seeds; 2013: 15 foraging events, 165 seeds) and Black Bulbul (Hypsipetes leucocephalus) (2011: 233 foraging events, 3728 seeds; 2012: 253 foraging events, 4048 seeds) were the most common foragers in the natural and patchy habitats, respectively. These birds were also the most common dispersers for T. chinensis, as determined by rates of seed removal from the three habitat types.

    In addition, seeds were removed and taken to greater distances from the farmland than from the other two habitats. Dispersal distance (farmland: 65 m; natural habitat: 50 m; patchy habitat: 60 m) and mean dispersal distance (farmland: 15.2 m; natural habitat: 8.1 m; patchy habitat: 13.8 m) for seeds from the farmland site were greater than those of the other two habitats (Fig. 1).

    Figure  1.  Patterns of seed dispersal distances of Taxus chinensis across three habitats (farmland, patchy habitat, natural habitat). Bars indicate mean values

    Considering the contributions of bird foraging traits to seed removal, the results of the random forest analysis showed 85.74% of seed removal data could be explained by eight variables: bird foraging frequency, weight, body length, food habit, site, year, habitat specialisation and foraging pattern. Only three variables (bird foraging frequency, food habit and site) significantly affected seed removal patterns. The results showed a clear positive association between bird foraging frequency and number of seeds removed (Fig. 2a). The contribution by frugivores to seed removal was higher than that by omnivorous birds (Fig. 2b), and more seeds were removed from the farmland site (Fig. 2c).

    Figure  2.  Relationships between seed removal and bird foraging traits (a, b) across three habitats (c). Results of a random forest analysis show the partial effects of independent variables on seed removal

    During field-based observations, a total of 687 seed dispersal distances were confirmed. Considering the contribution of bird flight traits to seed dispersal distance, 22.45% of seed dispersal distance could be explained by six variables (wing length, tail length, weight, body length, site and habitat specialisation). Three variables (bird tail length, wing length, and site) significantly affected seed removal patterns. The results of the random forest analysis demonstrated clear positive associations of bird tail (Fig. 3a) and wing length (Fig. 3b) with seed dispersal distance. The dispersal distance from the three habitat types differed, where the longest dispersal distances were observed at both the patchy habitat and the farmland site (Fig. 3c).

    Figure  3.  Relationship between seed dispersal distance with bird flight traits (a, b) across three habitats (c). Results of a random forest analysis show the partial effects of independent variables on seed dispersal distance

    To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to find that T. chinensis trees have the ability to form seed dispersal mutualisms with local birds across different disturbed habitats. As a consequence of these mutualisms, more seeds were removed by birds from the patchy habitat than from the other two habitats (Fig. 1). The number of seeds removed increased with bird foraging frequency (Fig. 2a). Moreover, the dispersal distances from the three habitats differed, where the longest dispersal distances were observed at both the patchy habitat and the farmland site (Fig. 3c). Seed dispersal distance increased with bird tail and wing length (Fig. 3a, b).

    There are several possible reasons to explain why more T. chinensis seeds were removed by birds at the patchy habitat, mostly involving the habitat features of the patchy habitat. First, the community structure of the patchy habitat was simple, which could attract more birds to forage and remove the seeds (Breitbach et al. 2010). In addition, the co-fruiting plants found in the patchy habitat may have helped to attract birds to the vicinity of the focal trees (Schupp et al. 2010). Moreover, the longer dispersal distance observed at the farmland site (Fig. 1) could have been related to the lack of perching trees in this disturbed habitat. As a consequence, birds had to fly farther to find trees to perch on (Cody 1985).

    Foraging behavioural traits strongly affected bird seed removal patterns across the three habitats (Fig. 2), which could be explained by both bird and plant traits (Farwig et al. 2017). For the bird perspective, it has been shown that medium-sized birds are not as sensitive to disturbance as large-bodied birds (Moore and Swihart 2007; Breitbach et al. 2010), as the remaining forests still provide adequate shelter and food for the persistence of medium-sized birds in disturbed habitats. For the plant perspective, plant traits of T. chinensis also determined the persistence of birds in disturbed habitats, thus affecting the foraging behaviour of birds (Farwig et al. 2017; Muñoz et al. 2017). Crop size, fruit size, and height of T. chinensis could be good advertisements that attract birds (crop size: 4000 per mother tree; fruit weight: 7 g; height: 25‒45 m) (Jordano and Schupp 2000; Martinez et al. 2008; Herrera and García 2009; Li et al. 2017). As a consequence, most of the bird species preferred to forage the "fruit" of this plant, and therefore chose the plant as their main food, which in turn determined the seed removal rate across the three habitats. Thus, the characteristics of the yew tree are a better predictor of seed removal rate in different habitats.

    Furthermore, our results showed that seed dispersal distance increased with bird tail and wing length in all three different habitats (Fig. 3). Wing length played an important role in determining the dispersal distance of the birds in the present study, and was an indicator of the role of the birds' flight ability in dispersal distance, as the birds' flight ability has been shown to increase with increasing wing length (Muñoz et al. 2017). Thus, wing length exerts a positive effect on seed dispersal distance. Our results also highlighted the interaction between bird tail length and seed dispersal distance. Tail length affects the ability to steer, and thus the forest specialisation of the birds. Birds with long tail length have been shown to exhibit both high tolerance to forest adaptation and high potential for flight (Fahrig 2007; Li et al. 2017), thus resulting in their long dispersal distances in all habitats.

    Our results highlight the importance of the functional traits of birds in the seed dispersal patterns of endangered trees across disturbed habitats. Bird behavioural traits could determine the ecological role of the species in seed removal, while morphological traits of birds may affect the level of the species-specific contribution to seed dispersal distance across disturbed habitats.

    NL, ZW and ZL designed the study. NL performed data collection. NL, and XL conducted all statistical analyses. All authors contributed to the writing and editing of the manuscript. All the authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

    We thank Zhongwei Zheng and Tianshi Xiong for their contributions in the field work.

    The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

    The datasets used in the present study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

    Not applicable.

    The experiments comply with the current laws of China in which they were performed.

  • Amrhein V, Erne N. Dawn singing reflects past territorial challenges in the winter wren. Anim Behav. 2006;71:1075-80.
    Amrhein V, Kunc HP, Naguib M. Seasonal patterns of singing activity vary with time of day in the nightingale Luscinia megarhynchos. Auk. 2004;121:110-7.
    Amrhein V, Johannessen LE, Kristiansen L, Slagsvold T. Reproductive strategy and singing activity: blue tit and great tit compared. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2008;62:1633-41.
    Bédard J, LaPointe G. Banding returns, arrival times, and site fidelity in the Savannah Sparrow. Wilson Bull. 1984;96:196-205.
    Bruni A, Foote JR. Dawn singing of Eastern phoebes varies with breeding stage and brood number. Wilson J Ornithol. 2014;126:500-7.
    Burt JM, Vehrencamp SL. Dawn chorus as an interactive communication network. In: McGregor P, editor. Animal communication networks. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2005. p. 320-43.
    Catchpole CK. The functions of advertising song in the Sedge Warbler Acrocephalus schoenobaenus and the Reed Warbler A. scirpaceus. Behaviour. 1973;46:300-19.
    Cuthill I, Hindmarsh A. Increase in starling song activity with removal of mate. Anim Behav. 1985;33:326-8.
    Cuthill IC, Macdonald WA. Experimental manipulation of the dawn and dusk chorus in the blackbird Turdus merula. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1990;26:209-16.
    Demko AD, Reitsma LR, Staicer CA. Two song categories in the Canada warbler Cardellina canadensis. Auk. 2013;130:609-16.
    Dixon CL. Breeding biology of the Savannah Sparrow on Kent Island. Auk. 1978;95:235-46.
    Erne N, Amrhein V. Long-term influence of simulated territorial intrusions on dawn and dusk singing in the Winter Wren: spring versus autumn. J Ornithol. 2008;149:479-86.
    Foote JR, Barber C. Paired male Song Sparrows sing more when their mate is incubating. Wilson J Ornithol. 2009;121:819-22.
    Foote JR, Fitzsimmons LP, Mennill DJ, Ratcliffe LM. Male black-capped chickadees begin dawn chorusing earlier in response to simulated territorial insertions. Anim Behav. 2011;81:871-7.
    Freeman-Gallant CR, Wheelwright NT, Meiklejohn KE, States SL, Sollecito SV. Little effect of extrapair paternity on the opportunity for sexual selection in Savannah Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis). Evolution. 2005;59:422-30.
    Freeman-Gallant CR, Wheewright NT, Meiklejohn KE, Sollecito SV. Genetic similarity, extrapair paternity, and offspring quality in Savannah Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis). Behav Ecol. 2006;17:952-8.
    Gil D, Graves JA, Slater PJB. Seasonal patterns of singing in the willow warbler: evidence against the fertility announcement hypothesis. Anim Behav. 1999;58:995-1000.
    Hanski I, Laurila A. Variation in song rate during the breeding cycle of the chaffinch, Fringilla coelebs. Ethology. 1993;93:161-9.
    Johnson LS, Kermott LH. The functions of song in male House Wrends Troglodytes aedon. Behaviour. 1991;166:190-209.
    Kacelnik A, Krebs JR. The dawn chorus in the great tit Parus major: proximate and ultimate causes. Behaviour. 1983;83:287-308.
    Kunc HP, Amrhein V, Naguib M. Seasonal variation in dawn song characteristics in the common nightingale. Anim Behav. 2005;70:1265-71.
    Lampe HM, Espmark YO. Singing activity and song pattern of the Redwing Turdus iliacus during the breeding season. Ornis Scand. 1987;18:179-85.
    Liu WC. The effect of neighbours and females on dawn and daytime singing behaviours by male chipping sparrows. Anim Behav. 2004;68:39-44.
    Mace R. The dawn chorus in the great tit Parus major is directly related to female fertility. Nature. 1987;330:745-6.
    Mennill DJ, Battiston M, Wilson DR, Foote JR, Doucet SM. Field test of an affordable, portable, wireless microphone array for spatial monitoring of animal ecology and behaviour. Methods Ecol Evol. 2012;3:704-12.
    Mennill DJ, Doucet SM, Newman AEM, Williams H, Moran IG, Thomas IP, et al. Wild birds learn songs from simulated tutors: the dynamics of vocal learning in nature. Curr Biol. 2018;28:3273-8.
    Møller AP. Why mated songbirds sing so much: mate guarding and male announcement of mate fertility status. Am Nat. 1991;138:994-1014.
    Murphy MT, Sexton K, Dolan AC, Redmond LJ. Dawn song of the eastern kingbird: An honest signal of male quality? Anim Behav. 2008;75:1075-84.
    Naguib M, van Rooij EP, Snijders L, Van Oers K. To sing or not to sing: seasonal changes in singing vary with personality in wild great tits. Behav Ecol. 2016;27:932-8.
    Oliveira DAG, Ades C. Long-distance calls in Neotropical primates. An Acad Bras Cienc. 2004;76:393-8.
    Ord TJ. Dawn and dusk "chorus" in visually communicating Jamaican anole lizards. Am Nat. 2008;172:585-92.
    Poesel A, Dabelsteen T, Darden S-K, Delhey K, Peters A. Territorial responses of male blue tits, Cyanistes caeruleus, to UV-manipulated neighbours. J Ornithol. 2006;148:179-87.
    Potter PE. Territorial behavior in Savannah Sparrows in southeastern Michigan. Wilson Bull. 1972;84:48-59.
    Reid ML, Weatherhead PJ. Mate-choice criteria of Ipswich sparrows: the importance of variability. Anim Behav. 1990;40:538-44.
    Runkle LS, Wells KD, Robb CC, Lance SL. Individual, nightly, and seasonal variation in call behavior of the gray tree frog, Hyla versicolor: implications for energy expenditure. Behav Ecol. 1994;5:318-25.
    Slagsvold T. Bird song activity in relation to breeding cycle, spring weather, and environmental phenology. Ornis Scand. 1977;8:197-222.
    Slagsvold T, Dale S, Sætre G-P. Dawn singing in the great tit Parus major: Mate attraction, mate guarding, or territorial defence? Behaviour. 1994;131:115-38.
    Staicer CA, Spector DA, Horn AG. The dawn chorus and other diel patterns in acoustic signalling. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH, editors. Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. New York: Cornell University Press; 1996. p. 426-53.
    Stafford KM, Nieukirk SL, Fox CG. Geographic and seasonal variation of blue whale calls in the North Pacific. J Cetacean Res Manag. 2001;3:65-76.
    Tufts RW. Birds of Nova Scotia. 3rd ed. Halifax: Nimbus Publ. Ltd. and Nova Scotia Mus; 1986.
    Wheelwright NT, Mauck RA. Philopatry, natal dispersal, and inbreeding avoidance in an island population of Savannah Sparrows. Ecology. 1998;79:755-67.
    Wheelwright N, Rising J. Savannah Sparrow Passerculus sandwichensis. The birds of North America, vol. 45. New York: Cornell University Press; 2008.
    Williams H, Levin II, Norris DR, Newman AEM, Wheelwright NT. Three decades of cultural evolution in Savannah sparrow songs. Anim Behav. 2013;85:213-23.
    Woodworth BK, Newman AEM, Turbek SP, Dossman BC, Jobson KA, Wassenaar LI, et al. Differential migration and the link between winter latitude, timing of migration and breeding in a songbird. Oecologia. 2016;181:413-22.
    Zhang VY, Celis-Murillo A, Ward MP. Conveying information with one song type: changes in dawn song performance correspond to different female breeding stages. Bioacoustics. 2016;25:19-28.
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(18)

    1. Jamie M. Douglas, Joanne Paul-Murphy, Elizabeth Stelow, et al. Personality Characteristics Predictive of Social Pairing Outcome in Orange-Winged Amazon Parrots ( Amazona amazonica ). Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 2024, 27(2): 386. DOI:10.1080/10888705.2023.2268522
    2. Ossi Nokelainen, Patrik Lauha, Sebastian Andrejeff, et al. A Mobile Application–Based Citizen Science Product to Compile Bird Observations. Citizen Science: Theory and Practice, 2024, 9(1) DOI:10.5334/cstp.710
    3. Felix Noel Estrada Piñero, Fernando Urbina, Alejandro A. Ríos-Chelén, et al. Vocal characterization of the Streak-backed Oriole (Icterus pustulatus) in Morelos, Mexico, using recordings from sound collections. The Wilson Journal of Ornithology, 2024, 136(1) DOI:10.1676/22-00104
    4. Sarah L. Dobney, Mikayla M.K. Bornais, D. Ryan Norris, et al. Quiet in the nest: The nest environment attenuates song in a grassland songbird. Avian Research, 2023, 14: 100148. DOI:10.1016/j.avrs.2023.100148
    5. Shauni E. T. Omond, John A. Lesku. Persistence of Nocturnality in Decapitated and Bisected Flatworms. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 2023, 38(3): 269. DOI:10.1177/07487304231158947
    6. Miguel A. Mejías, David R. Wilson. The relationships of breeding stage to daytime singing behaviour and song perch height in Bermuda white‐eyed vireos Vireo griseus bermudianus. Journal of Avian Biology, 2023, 2023(7-8) DOI:10.1111/jav.03116
    7. Lotte Schlicht, Emmi Schlicht, Peter Santema, et al. A dawn and dusk chorus will emerge if males sing in the absence of their mate. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 2023, 290(2011) DOI:10.1098/rspb.2023.2266
    8. Kaitlyn Plastino, Kevin C. Hannah, Rich Russell, et al. Hitting all the notes: Connecticut warblers sing an extended song type. Journal of Ornithology, 2022, 163(4): 953. DOI:10.1007/s10336-022-02005-z
    9. Juliana Rodríguez-Fuentes, Carlos Daniel Cadena, Jorge Enrique Avendaño. Vocal behavior and microgeographic variation in song types in a Neotropical songbird. Journal of Ornithology, 2022, 163(1): 105. DOI:10.1007/s10336-021-01930-9
    10. Abby L J Hensel, Sarah L Dobney, Ines G Moran, et al. Passive acoustic monitoring provides predictable and reliable underestimates of population size and longevity in wild Savannah Sparrows. Ornithological Applications, 2022, 124(3) DOI:10.1093/ornithapp/duac018
    11. Sanja Barišić, Davor Ćiković, Vesna Tutiš, et al. Context dependent song-flight performance and timing in the Black-headed Bunting (Emberiza melanocephala). Avian Research, 2022, 13: 100059. DOI:10.1016/j.avrs.2022.100059
    12. Abby L.J. Hensel, Sarah L. Dobney, Stéphanie M. Doucet, et al. Microgeographical variation in birdsong: Savannah sparrows exhibit microdialects in an island population. Animal Behaviour, 2022, 188: 119. DOI:10.1016/j.anbehav.2022.04.008
    13. Parker Rundstrom, Nicole Creanza. Song learning and plasticity in songbirds. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 2021, 67: 228. DOI:10.1016/j.conb.2021.02.003
    14. Michał Budka, Patryk Kokociński. Daily and seasonal changes of vocal activity of the Common Crane Grus grus: implications for conservation and monitoring efforts. Bird Study, 2021, 68(3): 311. DOI:10.1080/00063657.2022.2032590
    15. Johann H van Niekerk, Giovanni Forcina. Purpose of Crested FrancolinOrtygornis sephaenamale collective calls at dusk based on livestream audio censusing. Ostrich, 2020, 91(4): 326. DOI:10.2989/00306525.2020.1827463
    16. Mohammad Fahmy, David R. Wilson. Singing behaviour of ruby‐crowned kinglets Regulus calendula in relation to time‐of‐day, time‐of‐year and social context. Journal of Avian Biology, 2020, 51(7) DOI:10.1111/jav.02467
    17. Megan J. Thompson, Kendriah A. Pearse, Jennifer R. Foote, et al. Seasonal and diel plasticity of song type use in individual ovenbirds (Seiurus aurocapilla). Ethology, 2020, 126(8): 824. DOI:10.1111/eth.13040
    18. Daniel J. Mennill, Stéphanie M. Doucet, Amy E.M. Newman, et al. Eavesdropping on adult vocal interactions does not enhance juvenile song learning: an experiment with wild songbirds. Animal Behaviour, 2019, 155: 67. DOI:10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.05.019

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(4)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (278) PDF downloads (7) Cited by(18)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return