Tomasz S. Osiejuk, Bartłomiej Żbikowski, Amie Wheeldon, Michał Budka. 2019: Hey mister Tambourine Dove, sing a song for me: simple but individually specific songs of Turtur tympanistria from Cameroon. Avian Research, 10(1): 14. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-019-0153-x
Citation:
Tomasz S. Osiejuk, Bartłomiej Żbikowski, Amie Wheeldon, Michał Budka. 2019: Hey mister Tambourine Dove, sing a song for me: simple but individually specific songs of Turtur tympanistria from Cameroon. Avian Research, 10(1): 14. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-019-0153-x
Tomasz S. Osiejuk, Bartłomiej Żbikowski, Amie Wheeldon, Michał Budka. 2019: Hey mister Tambourine Dove, sing a song for me: simple but individually specific songs of Turtur tympanistria from Cameroon. Avian Research, 10(1): 14. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-019-0153-x
Citation:
Tomasz S. Osiejuk, Bartłomiej Żbikowski, Amie Wheeldon, Michał Budka. 2019: Hey mister Tambourine Dove, sing a song for me: simple but individually specific songs of Turtur tympanistria from Cameroon. Avian Research, 10(1): 14. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-019-0153-x
The broadcast song of the Tambourine Dove (Turtur tympanistria) is a repetitive coo series produced in a very stereotyped way. The song is similar to those of the other species within the Turtur genus and likely reflects the adaptation to long distance signalling in tropical habitats. There is no study assessing individual variation of the Tambourine Dove song or for the possibility of identity coding through song features.
Methods
We obtained recordings of 578 songs from 41 males within a population inhabiting montane forest in Cameroon between 2008 and 2018. Additionally, we analysed also recordings from automatic recorders working for a longer time during the year. Songs were analysed to get basic information about within- and between-individual spectro-temporal variation. Potential of identity coding and discriminant analyses were used to investigate if and how these songs may convey information about male identity.
Results
The song of the Tambourine Dove is a very simple series of short, unmodulated whistles with low frequency and a narrow bandwidth. Measurements of single song features indicated either none or a weak potential for identity coding. The overall variation in song frequency characteristics was very low. However, the number of notes forming a song (and consequently the duration) had a similar variability both within- and between individuals. When using multiple song measurements discrimination analysis revealed a high potential for identity coding although it seems that the temporal pattern of the beginning of a song carries the most information about sender identity. The production of initial notes within a song has very precise, individually specific timing allowing for up to 96.7% correct recognitions of songs. Comparison of recordings from the same points but separated in time by over a month indicates that song parameters are individually stable over longer period.
Conclusions
Despite having highly stereotyped and simple songs, Tambourine Dove males produce individually specific signals which have a great potential for recognition by rivals or mates. All Turtur doves have similar yet unique songs that are different to the songs of other pigeon species. This genus seems to be a perfect model for studying the trade-off between coding for species identity and individual identity in a non-learning bird species. The described method of individual recognition allows for nonintrusive population monitoring.
Interactions between avian obligate brood parasites and
their hosts remain one of the most robust examples of
coevolutionary arms races (Davies, 2000;
Stoddard and Stevens, 2010; Kilner and Langmore, 2011). The best
studied and historically most prominent example of
such interactions is the evolved mimicry of host eggs
by parasites (Moksnes and Røskaft 1995; Cherry et al., 2007a; Moskát et al., 2008, 2010;
Spottiswoode and Stevens, 2010; Soler et al., 2012). Despite the extensive
similarities in the appearance of host and parasitic eggs
(Grim, 2005), many host species possess the ability
to discriminate between own and foreign eggs (Stoddard and Stevens, 2011). Much attention has recently
been given to the functional roles of light wavelengths
beyond the human perceptual range in avian egg discrimination, including the role of the shorter, ultraviolet
(UV) wavelengths (< 400 nm) (e.g. Honza et al., 2007), to which different species of birds within distantly related lineages are varyingly sensitive (e.g. Ödeen and
Håstad, 2003; Machovsky Capuska et al., 2011; Aidala
et al., 2012). For example, UV-reflectance is important
in recognizing and rejecting foreign eggs in the Blackcap (Sylvia atricapilla) (Honza and Polačiková, 2008)
and the Song Thrush (Turdus philomelos) (Honza et al., 2007). However, comparatively less emphasis has been
given to describing the visual sensitivities, UV or otherwise, of avian obligate brood parasites themselves.
Describing the visual sensitivities of specific bird species is vital, especially because the avian visual world
differs substantially from that of humans. For example, unlike trichromatic humans, who possess only three
classes of cone photoreceptor, birds possess five classes, four of which are directly responsible for color perception (Hunt et al., 2009). The short wavelength-sensitive
type 1 (SWS1) photoreceptor, which is responsible for
short-wavelength light detection, differs in its maximal
sensitivity depending on the amino acids present at
key 'spectral tuning' sites 86, 90, and 93 (following the
bovine Bos taurus rhodopsin numbering) (Wilkie et al., 2000; Yokoyama et al., 2000; Shi et al., 2001). Of these, amino acid residue 90 is particularly important for
mediating the degree of UV-sensitivity in avian species
(Wilkie et al., 2000; Hunt et al., 2009). Those species
possessing serine at site 90 (S90) are designated as having violet-sensitive (VS) pigments with a maximal sensitivity > 400 nm, and those possessing cysteine (C)90
are designated as having UV-sensitive (UVS) pigments
with a maximal sensitivity < 400 nm (Hart, 2001). Site
90 is also highly conserved, with S90 proposed to be
the ancestral state in all birds (Yokoyama and Shi, 2000;
Hunt et al., 2009), though recent analyses of basal paleognaths (which were not included in these earlier analyses) including extinct moa from New Zealand, predicted a uniform UVS SWS1 for all ratites and tinamou
allies (Aidala et al., 2012). Therefore, it is likely that C90
has (re-)evolved independently several times among
avian lineages (Hunt et al., 2009; Ödeen et al., 2010;
Machovsky Capuska et al., 2011; Ödeen et al., 2011, Aidala et al. 2012). Because microspectrophotometric
and genetic data are in accord with one another in avian
taxa for which both types of data are available (i.e. those
possessing S90 have VS SWS1 opsins and those possessing C90 have UVS SWS1 opsins), DNA sequencing of
the SWS1 opsin gene therefore permits accurate assessment of the degree of UV-sensitivity in any given avian
species (Ödeen and Håstad, 2003) before the need for
invasive and terminal physiological experimentation
to confirm the sequence-based predictions (Aidala and Hauber, 2010).
Much of the work on the functional role of UVreflectance and sensitivity in brood parasitic birds has
focused on explaining the lack of eggshell color-based
egg rejection to seemingly non-mimetic parasitic eggs.
Cherry and Bennett's (2001) UV-matching hypothesis
suggests that matching host/parasitic egg reflectance
along a UV-green opponency (which humans cannot
see) may explain the lack of rejection in acceptor host
species. Empirical support for this hypothesis, however, is equivocal. For example, blocking-the UV-reflectance
of Great-spotted Cuckoo (Clamator glandarius) eggs
does not affect rejection in Common Magpies (Pica
pica) (Avilés et al., 2006). However, the UVS/VS SWS1
sensitivity in this parasite-impacted host species has
not been described, although other Corvidae species
are predicted to be VS based on SWS1 DNA sequencing
(Ödeen and Håstad, 2003). More critically, no apparent
relationship between accepter/rejecter status and UVS/
VS SWS1 sensitivity appears to exist among hosts of the
North American generalist brood parasite, the Brownheaded Cowbird (Molothrus ater) and many of its hosts
(Underwood and Sealy, 2008; Aidala et al., 2012).
The degree of UV egg
color-matching/UV light sensitivity in New Zealand obligate brood parasite-host
systems is not yet described using reflectance spectrophotometric or avian perceptual
modeling data. The endemic Grey Warbler (Gerygone igata) is an ac-cepter
host of the local subspecies of the native Shin-ing Cuckoo (in Australia, called the Shining-bronze Cuckoo; Chalcites [Chrysococcyx] lucidus)
(McLean and Waas, 1987; also reviewed in Grim, 2006). In turn, the Whitehead (Mohoua
albicilla), Yellowhead (M. ochro-cephala), and Brown Creeper (M.
novaeseelandiae) are endemic hosts of the also endemic Long-tailed
Cuckoo (Urodynamis [Eudynamis] taitensis) (Payne, 2005). The Whitehead
and Yellowhead are both considered accept-er hosts (McLean and Waas, 1987;
Briskie, 2003), while the Brown Creeper ejects artificial Long-tailed Cuckoo
eggs at a rate of 67% (Briskie, 2003). DNA sequencing of the SWS1 photoreceptor
in the Grey Warbler and the Whitehead predicted a VS and a UVS SWS1 maximal
sensitivity, respectively (Aidala et al., 2012), whereas the predicted
sensitivities of their respective parasites are not well known.
Compared
to the large amount of effort spent char-acterizing the visual sensitivities of
host species, those of brood parasites themselves, especially to UV-wavelengths, have received considerably less attention. To
date, the SWS1 sensitivities have not been described in
any Cuculiformes species, although a study measuring
UV-reflectance in feather patches of 24 of 143 (17%)
total cuckoo species showed that 5 of the species (21%
of those measured) showed peaks in UV-reflectance
(Mullen and Pohland, 2008). As there are increasingly
more known inter-and intra-order variations in avian
UV-sensitivity (Ödeen and Håstad, 2003;
Machovsky Capuska et al., 2011; Aidala et al., 2012; Ödeen et al., 2012), and because visual systems among closely related
species may vary widely, and are likely to reflect speciesspecific sensory ecologies (Machovsky Capuska et al., 2012), reliance on species for which SWS1 sensitivity
data are available even within a lineage to approximate
the degree of UV-sensitivity may be inaccurate.
Characterization
of the UV-sensitivities of brood parasitic species is important for several
reasons. First, it will allow for stronger analysis of comparative per-ceptual
coevolution between hosts and parasites (An-derson et al., 2009). For example, recent egg color work using spectrophotometric measurements across the entire
avian visible range have provided new insights into the direction of
coevolutionary processes between hosts and parasites. Great Reed Warblers (Acrocephalus
arundinaceus) are more likely to reject mimetic Com-mon Cuckoo (Cuculus
canorus) eggs when this hosts?own eggs exhibit higher intraclutch
variation, a finding not in line with traditional predictions of
coevolution-ary theory, but validated by spectrophotometric mea-surements of
host eggs (Cherry et al., 2007a; see also Antonov et al., 2012). Similarly, Common Cuckoos may preferentially parasitize host nests with eggs more closely
resembling their own, also out of line with the theoretical assumption that
female cuckoos randomly choose local nests to parasitize (Cherry et al., 2007b). Second, describing the visual sensitivities of brood parasitic cuckoo
species will better inform studies ex-amining cuckoo-cuckoo competition
(Brooker et al., 1990) over host nesting sites using visual modeling analyses.
Third, it will allow for more accurate analysis of VS/UVS SWS1 opsin ancestral
states among avian species (Hunt et al., 2009; Aidala et al., 2012). Here, we
report the predicted maximal sensitivities of the SWS1 opsins in two New
Zealand native brood parasitic cuck-oos based on DNA sequencing of the SWS1 'pectral
tuning?region. In keeping with the general theoretical framework that host egg
rejection selects for egg color matching, and in turn, favors
UV-sensitivity in hosts, which in turn selects for UV-sensitivity in parasites, we expect the Shining Cuckoo that parasitizes the VS-pre-dicted Grey Warbler to
possess VS SWS1 opsins and the Long-tailed Cuckoo that parasitizes the
UVS-predicted Whitehead to possess UVS SWS1 opsins.
Methods
We collected ~100 μL blood samples that were stored
in Queen's lysis buffer from live Shining Cuckoos captured in mistnets during our field studies on avian hostparasite interactions (Anderson et al., 2009). We also
obtained tissue samples from frozen Long-tailed Cuckoos that died from migration-related window-collisions
and were stored in the Auckland Museum collection
(Gill and Hauber, 2012). Our collecting protocols were
approved by governmental and institutional animal research committees. Total genomic DNA was extracted
from tissue samples stored in ethanol using the DNeasy
Blood and Tissue Kit (Qiagen) according to manufacturer's instructions. DNA concentration (ng·μL–1) was
estimated using Nanodrop spectrophotometer.
Forward primers SU149a (Shining Cuckoo) or SU193
(Long-tailed Cuckoo) and reverse primer SU306b
(Ödeen and Håstad, 2003), modified to include M13-
tails, were used to sequence the SWS1 opsin gene. PCR
amplifications were carried out in 25 μL reaction volumes of 60 mmol·L–1 Tris-HCl ph 8.5, 15 mmol·L–1
(NH4)2SO4, 2.5 mmol·L–1 MgCl2, 0.3 mmol·L–1 of each
dNTP, 0.2 μmol·L–1 of each primer and 0.5 U of Platinum Taq polymerase (Invitrogen). Thermal cycling followed conditions outlined in Ödeen and Håstad (2003)
and was conducted in an ABI GeneAmp 9700 thermocycler.
An Exo/SAP treatment was used to purify PCR products: 5 μL PCR product was added to 0.2 μL of Exo I (GE
Healthcare), 0.1 μL Shrimp Alkaline Phosphatase (GE
Healthcare) and 1.7 μL UltraPure water (Invitrogen). We
incubated mixtures for 30 min at 37℃, then for 15 min
at 80℃ to ensure enzyme inactivation. A BigDye Terminator Cycle Sequencing kit v3.1 (Applied Biosystems)
was used to sequence samples in both directions with
M13 forward and reverse primers. Each sequencing reaction consisted of 1 μL BigDye Terminator Mix, 3.5 μL
5× sequencing buffer, 0.2 μmol·L–1 primer, 1 μL DMSO
and 2 μL PCR product. Agencourt CleanSeq (Beckman
Coulter) was used according to manufacturer's instructions to purify sequencing reactions and analyzed using an ABI 3100 automated sequencer. Chromas Pro (Technelysium Pty. Ltd.) was used to edit sequences following
which they were exported to BioEdit (Hall, 1999) for
alignment and translation.
Results
The two Shining Cuckoo samples generated a sequence
length of 119 base pairs (bp) each. The two Long-tailed
Cuckoo samples generated a sequence length of 74 bp
each. All sequences have been made available on GenBank (Accession numbers HM159121–HM159124).
We detected no intraspecific or intrafamilial variation
in either the gene or amino acid sequences, except for
the codons at residue 95; however, both of these code
for the amino acid phenylalanine (Table 1). We found
only two ambiguities in one Long-tailed Cuckoo sample, whereas the other Long-tailed Cuckoo possessed
the same codons and amino acid residues as the two
Shining Cuckoo samples (Table 1). After alignment, all
samples possessed S86, S90, and T93, which predict VS
for both of these cuckoo species' SWS1 opsin photoreceptors.
Table
1.
Predicted VS/UVS SWS1 opsin state of two New Zealand cuckoo species based on SWS1 amino acid sequences. Passerine
host species are shown below each cuckoo species and were adapted from Aidala et al. (2012). Spectral tuning sites 86, 90, and 93 are
underlined.
This is the first study to report on the sequence of SWS1
receptors and to predict short-wavelength visual sensitivities of New Zealand's brood parasitic native Shining
Cuckoos and endemic Long-tailed Cuckoos. Substituting S for A at amino acid residue 86 (A86S substitution)
produces a short-wave shift of 1 nm, a T93V substitution produces a long-wave shift of 3 nm, and a C90S
substitution produces a 35 nm long-wave shift in the
UVS SWS1 opsin of the Budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus) (Wilkie et al. 2000). The same C90S substitution in the Zebra Finch (Taeniopygia guttata) produces
a similar-magnitude long-wave shift of SWS1 maximal
sensitivity from 359 to 397 nm (Yokoyama et al., 2000).
Thus, despite possessing S86 and T93 in both species, the presence of S90 predicts that the SWS1 maximal
sensitivities of our cuckoo samples should be well within the visible-violet portion of the light spectrum, or VS
(Table 1).
This finding is contradictory to our original prediction that only the Long-tailed Cuckoo should possess
UVS SWS1 opsins due to the predicted UVS SWS1 of
its Whitehead host (in contrast with the VS SWS1 of
the Shining Cuckoo's Grey Warbler host; Table 1). Accordingly, we did not observe a distinct pattern between
predicted SWS1 sensitivities of our cuckoo samples and
those of their hosts. Both the Grey Warbler and Whitehead are non-ejector hosts of the Shining and Longtailed Cuckoos respectively, yet these host species differ
in their predicted SWS1 maximal sensitivities; DNA
sequencing of the SWS1 photoreceptor gene predicted
a VS SWS1 in the Grey Warbler but a UVS SWS1 in the
Whitehead (Aidala et al., 2012). Predicted sensitivities of the other two Long-tailed Cuckoo hosts, the
non-ejector Yellowhead, and the artificial egg-ejecting
Brown Creeper are not yet described from molecular
sequencing data. Also undocumented is the degree of
physical or perceptual host-parasite egg color matching, in the UV-portion specifically, and in the avianvisible spectrum overall, in these two host-parasite systems. Nonetheless, human-visible assessment suggests some level of mimicry between Long-tailed Cuckoos
and their hosts (Briskie, 2003), whereas the dark Shining
Cuckoo's eggs may be cryptic, and not mimetic, in the
enclosed nests of the Grey Warbler hosts (see Langmore
et al., 2009).
An alternative to perceptual coevolutionary processes mediating the detection of parasitic eggs in New
Zealand hosts is that the cost of accepting parasitic eggs
might be offset by recognizing and rejecting parasitic
cuckoo chicks (Davies, 2000). Despite a lack of direct
behavioral or sensory data in our focal systems, there is
evidence of parasitic chick detection and ejection based
on visual appearance in the closely related Australian
Large-billed Gerygone (Gerygone manirostris)/Little
Bronze-cuckoo (Chalcites [Chrysococcyx] minutillus)
(Sato et al., 2010) and Superb Fairy-wren (Malurus cyaneus)/Shining Cuckoo host-parasite systems (Langmore
et al., 2003; see also Langmore et al., 2011). Further, there is evidence of evolved call-matching of the begging calls of Grey Warblers by Shining Cuckoo chicks
based on both sound recordings (McLean and Waas, 1987) and comparative phylogenetic inference (Anderson et al., 2009). Similarly, McLean and Waas (1987)
noted and Ranjard et al. (2010) provided bioacoustic
evidence for the evolved similarity between the begging
calls of the Long-tailed Cuckoo and its Mohoua spp.
hosts. Other parasitic cuckoo-host systems, including
the Horsefield's Bronze-cuckoo (Chalcites [Chrysococcyx] basalis) and its Superb Fairy-wren (Malurus cyaneus) hosts (Langmore et al., 2003, 2008; Colombelli-Negrel et al., 2012), the Diederick Cuckoo (Chrysococcyx
caprius) and its hosts, and the Koel (Eudynamis scolopacea) and its House Crow (Corvus splendens) hosts have
also been shown to have similar begging calls (reviewed
in Grim, 2006).
Characterizing the visual sensitivities of diverse avian
lineages, including parasitic cuckoo species, is an important step in understanding the coevolution of visual
perception/parasitic egg rejection behaviors in hostparasite interactions and sensory ecology. These studies
form the basis for future visual modeling and sensoryphysiological studies for more accurate description of
the perceptual systems of focal cuckoo species. Future
studies should investigate the behavioral significance
of egg color matching in driving sensory coevolution
using appropriate visual perceptual modeling analyses
of both host and parasitic species (Aidala and Hauber, 2010). Additional Cuculiformes species should also be
included in future analyses in order to better describe
the degree of V/UV-matching in host-parasite egg color
mimicry and its perception and the ecological variables
that may drive or hinder the evolution of UV-sensitivity
amongst parasitic and non-parasitic cuckoos (Krüger et al., 2009).
Acknowledgments
All field work was conducted in accordance
with local animal ethics rules and regulations in New Zealand
and the University of Auckland. We thank Brian Gill for
providing tissue samples of Long-tailed Cuckoos at the Auckland
Museum and Andrew Fidler for advice on sequencing. We also
thank the many volunteers for assistance in field work, and
two anonymous reviewers for the helpful comments on our
manuscript. This research was funded by the US National Science
Foundation and the Graduate Center of the City University of
New York (to ZA and to MEH), a Foundation for Research, Science, and Technology postdoctoral fellowship (to MGA), and
the National Geographic Society, the PSC-CUNY grant scheme, and the Human Frontier Science Program (to MEH).
Ay E, Fischer J. The "Acoustic Adaptation Hypothesis"—a review of the evidence from birds, anurans and mammals. Bioacoustics. 2009;19:21-48.
Ballintijn MR, ten Cate C. Vocal development and its differentiation in a non-songbird: the collared dove (Streptopelia decaocto). Behaviour. 1997a;134:595-621.
Ballintijn MR, ten Cate C. Sex differences in the vocalisations and syrinx of the collared dove (Streptopelia decaocto). Auk. 1997b;22:22-39.
Ballintijn MR, ten Cate C. Sound production in the collared dove: a test of the 'whistle' hypothesis. J Exp Biol. 1998;201:1637-49.
Baptista LF, Trail PW, Horblit HM, Kirwan GM. Tambourine Dove (Turtur tympanistria). In: del Hoyo J, Elliott A, Sargatal J, Christie DA, de Juana E, editors. Handbook of the birds of the world alive. Barcelona: Lynx Edicions; 2018a. . Accessed 30 Aug 2018.
Baptista LF, Trail PW, Horblit HM. Pigeons, Doves (Columbidae). In: del Hoyo J, Elliott A, Sargatal J, Christie DA, de Juana E, editors. Handbook of the birds of the world alive. Barcelona: Lynx Edicions; 2018b. . Accessed 30 Aug 2018.
Beckers GJL, ten Cate C. Perceptual relevance of species-specific differences in acoustic signal structure in Streptopelia doves. Anim Behav. 2001;62:511-8.
Beckers GJL, Suthers RA, ten Cate C. Mechanisms of frequency and amplitude modulation in ring dove song. J Exp Biol. 2003a;206:1833-43.
Beckers GJL, Suthers RA, ten Cate C. Pure-tone birdsong by resonance filtering of harmonic overtones. PNAS. 2003b;100:7372-6.
Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL. Principles of animal communication. 2nd ed. Sunderland: Sinauer Press; 2011.
Briefer E, Aubin T, Lehongre K, Rybak F. How to identify dear enemies: the group signature in the complex song of the skylark Alauda arvensis. J Exp Biol. 2008;211:317-26.
Budka M, Osiejuk TS. Individually specific call feature is not used to neighbor-stranger discrimination: the corncrake case. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e104031.
Budka M, Osiejuk TS. Neighbour-stranger call discrimination in a nocturnal rails species, the Corncrake Crex crex. J Ornithol. 2013;154:685-94.
Budka M, Deoniziak K, Tumiel T, Woźna JT. Vocal individuality in drumming in great spotted woodpecker - A biological perspective and implications for conservation. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(2):e0191716.
Charrier I, Mathevon N, Jouventin P. Individual identity coding depends on call type in the South Polar Skua Catharacta maccormicki. Polar Biol. 2001;24:378-82.
Catchpole CK, Slater PJB. Bird song: biological themes and variations. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2008.
de Kort SR, ten Cate C. Response to interspecific vocalisations is affected by degree of phylogenetic relatedness in Streptopelia doves. Anim Behav. 2001;61:239-47.
de Kort SR, den Hartog PM, ten Cate C. Diverge or merge? The effect of sympatric occurrence on the territorial vocalizations of the vinaceous dove Streptopelia vinacea and the ring-necked dove S. capicola. J Avian Biol. 2002a;33:150-8.
de Kort SR, den Hartog PM, ten Cate C. Vocal signals, isolation and hybridization in the vinaceous dove (Streptopelia vinacea) and the ring-necked dove (S. capicola). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2002b;51:378-85.
de Kort SR, ten Cate C. Repeated decrease in vocal repertoire size in Streptopelia doves. Anim Behav. 2004;67:549-57.
den Hartog PM, Slabbekoorn H, ten Cate C. Male territorial vocalizations and responses are decoupled in an avian hybrid zone. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2008;363:2879-89.
Draganoiu TI, Moreau A, Ravaux L, Bonckaert W, Mathevon N. Song stability and neighbour recognition in a migratory songbird, the black redstart. Behaviour. 2014;151:435-53.
Elemans CPH, Spierts ILY, Muller UK, van Leeuwen JL, Goller F. Superfast muscles control dove's trill. Nature. 2004;431:146.
Elemans CPH, Zaccarelli R, Herzel H. Biomechanics and control of vocalization in a non-songbird. J R Soc Interface. 2008;5:691-703.
Fitzsimmons LP, Barker NK, Mennill DJ. Individual variation and lek-based vocal distinctiveness in songs of the screaming piha (Lipagus vociferans), a suboscine songbird. Auk. 2008;125:908-14.
Fusani L. Steroid-sensitive mechanisms of vocal behaviour in the ring dove. MPhil Thesis. Cambridge, UK: University of Cambridge; 1994.
Hutchison RE, Hutchison JB, Fusani L. Vocal-postural co-ordination of a sexually dimorphic display in a monomorphic species: the Barbary dove. Behaviour. 1997;134:321-35.
Gil D, Gahr M. The honesty of bird song: multiple constraints for multiple traits. TREE. 2002;17:133-41.
Hardouin LA, Tabel P, Bretagnole V. Neighbour-stranger discrimination in the little owl, Athene noctua. Anim Behav. 2006;72:105-12.
Hitchcock RR, Mirarchi RE, Lishak RS. Recognition of individual male parent vocalisations by nestling mourning doves. Anim Behav. 1989;37:517-20.
Khan HA, Arif IA. COI barcodes and phylogeny of doves (Columbidae family). Mitochondr DNA. 2013;24:689-96.
Lade BI, Thorpe WH. Dove songs as innately coded patterns of specific behaviour. Nature. 1964;202:366-8.
Lovell SF, Lein MR. Neighbor-stranger discrimination by song in a suboscine bird, the alder flycatcher, Empidonax alnorum. Behav Ecol. 2004;15:799-804.
Mairy F. Les vocalisations de la Tourterelle rieuse domestique, Streptopelia risoria (L.): aspects structuraux, causaux, sémantiques et phonatoires. PhD Thesis. Belgium: Universite Liége. 1977.
Mindlin GB, Laje R. The physics of birdsong. Berlin: Springer; 2005. p. 157.
Morris RL, Erickson CJ. Pair bond maintenance in the ring dove (Streptopelia risoria). Anim Behav. 1971;19:398-406.
Orme CDL, Davies RFG, Burgess M, Eigenbrod F, Pickup N, Olson VA, Webster AJ, Ding TS, Rasmussen PC, Ridgely RS, Stattersfield AJ, Bennett PM, Blackburn TM, Gaston KJ, Owens IP. Global hotspot of species richness are not congruent with endemism of threat. Nature. 2005;436:1016-9.
Radford AN. Group-specific vocal signatures and neighbour-stranger discrimination in the cooperatively breeding green woodhoopoe. Anim Behav. 2005;70:1227-34.
Reif J, Horak D, Sedlacek O, Riegert J, Pesata M, Hrazsky Z, Janecek S, Storch D. Unusual abundance-range size relationship in an Afromontane bird community: the effect of geographical isolation? J Biogeogr. 2006;33:1959-68.
Stoddard PK. Vocal recognition of neighbors by territorial passerines. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH, editors. Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. Ithaca: Comstock/Cornell University Press; 1996. p. 356-76.
Suthers RA. How birds sing and why it matters. In: Marler P, Slabbekoorn H, editors. Nature's music: the science of birdsong. New York: Elsevier; 2004. p. 272-95.
ten Cate C, Ballintijn MR. Dove coos and flashed lights: interruptibility of "song" in a nonsongbird. J Comp Psychol. 1996;110:267-75.
Terpstra NJ, Bolhuis JJ, den Boer-Visser AM, ten Cate C. Neuronal activation related to auditory perception in the brain of a non-songbird, the ring dove. J Comp Neurobiol. 2005;488:342-51.
Tibbetts EA, Dale J. Individual recognition: is it good to be different? TREE. 2007;22:529-37.
Trivers RL. The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol. 1971;46:35-57.
Tubaro P, Mahler B. Acoustic frequencies and body mass in new world doves. Condor. 1998;100:54-61.
Warner RW. The anatomy of the syrinx in passerine birds. J Zool. 1972a;168:381-93.
Warner RW. The syrinx in family Columbidae. J Zool. 1972b;166:385-90.
Wiley RH. Individuality in songs of Acadian flycatchers and recognition of neighbours. Anim Behav. 2005;70:237-47.
Wolfenden A, Jones CG, Tatayah V, Zuel N, de Kort SR. Endangered pink pigeons treat calls of the ubiquitous Madagascan turtle dove as conspecific. Anim Behav. 2015;99:83-8.
Paweł Szymański, Małgorzata Niśkiewicz, Michał Budka, et al. Year-round vocal activity of the Tambourine Dove Turtur tympanistria in a seasonal Afrotropical mountain environment. Tropical Ecology, 2025.
DOI:10.1007/s42965-025-00369-w
2.
Małgorzata Niśkiewicz, Paweł Szymański, Lia Zampa, et al. Neighbour–stranger discrimination in an African wood dove inhabiting equatorial rainforest. Scientific Reports, 2024, 14(1)
DOI:10.1038/s41598-024-53867-7
3.
Pedro Diniz, Edvaldo F Silva-Jr, Gianlucca S Rech, et al. Duets convey information about pair and individual identities in a Neotropical bird. Current Zoology, 2024.
DOI:10.1093/cz/zoae064
4.
Małgorzata Niśkiewicz, Paweł Szymański, Michał Budka, et al. Response of forest Turtur doves to conspecific and congeneric songs in sympatry and allopatry. Scientific Reports, 2023, 13(1)
DOI:10.1038/s41598-023-43035-8
5.
Mónika Jablonszky, David Canal, Gergely Hegyi, et al. Estimating heritability of song considering within-individual variance in a wild songbird: The collared flycatcher. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 2022, 10
DOI:10.3389/fevo.2022.975687
6.
Vlastimil Hart, Richard Policht, Vojtěch Jandák, et al. Low frequencies in the display vocalization of the Western Capercaillie (Tetrao urogallus). PeerJ, 2020, 8: e9189.
DOI:10.7717/peerj.9189
Table
1.
Predicted VS/UVS SWS1 opsin state of two New Zealand cuckoo species based on SWS1 amino acid sequences. Passerine
host species are shown below each cuckoo species and were adapted from Aidala et al. (2012). Spectral tuning sites 86, 90, and 93 are
underlined.