Wenjing Wan, LizhiZhou, Yunwei Song. 2016: Shifts in foraging behavior of wintering Hooded Cranes (Grus monacha) in three different habitats at Shengjin Lake, China. Avian Research, 7(1): 13. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-016-0047-0
Citation: Wenjing Wan, LizhiZhou, Yunwei Song. 2016: Shifts in foraging behavior of wintering Hooded Cranes (Grus monacha) in three different habitats at Shengjin Lake, China. Avian Research, 7(1): 13. DOI: 10.1186/s40657-016-0047-0

Shifts in foraging behavior of wintering Hooded Cranes (Grus monacha) in three different habitats at Shengjin Lake, China

More Information
  • Corresponding author:

    Lizhi Zhou, zhoulz@ahu.edu.cn

  • Received Date: 19 Mar 2016
  • Accepted Date: 19 Jul 2016
  • Available Online: 24 Apr 2022
  • Publish Date: 16 Aug 2016
  • Background 

    Wetland loss and degradation result in a reduction in the availability and quality of food for wintering waterbirds. Birds normally modify their foraging behavior to adapt to variations in food availability. In this study, we compared shifts in foraging behavior of Hooded Cranes (Grus monacha) in three different habitats at Shengjin Lake, China to understand the response of these cranes to changes in habitat.

    Methods 

    We investigated the food density and foraging behavior of Hooded Cranes in Shengjin Lake National Nature Reserve from November 2014 to April 2015. We used regression equations to describe the changes in food density. A total of 397 behavioral observations were used in the analyses of their foraging efforts. We fitted a candidate set of generalized mixed linear models to analyze the relationship of foraging efforts and food density. We used a method of information theory to guide the selection of the model and Akaike's Information Criterion to calculate the value of each model. The relationship between food density, disturbances and foraging behavior was illustrated using a generalized linear model.

    Results 

    Along with the temporal variation and exploitation of food biomass, the food density varied widely among foraging sites. During the early winter period, foraging efforts were more pronounced in the paddy fields and meadows but not significantly different among the three habitats. The cranes spent more foraging effort in the paddy fields and meadows during the middle stage and in the meadows and mudflats during the late winter. The results of the generalized linear model showed that food density and disturbances had different effects on the rate of foraging success during the winter, while the effect of foraging effort was not significant. Furthermore, the rate of feeding success was markedly affected by disturbances in the paddy fields. The combined action of food density and disturbances had a significant effect on the rate of foraging success in the meadows, while the effect of foraging effort was also not significant in three habitats.

    Conclusions 

    Changes in foraging behavior were significant in three habitats, which were affected by food density and disturbances. The rate of foraging success increased in the habitat with low food density and low disturbances to increase the foraging efficiency in the lake. With abundant food and a high level of disturbance, the rate of foraging success decreased to ensure more secure access to food.

  • In days gone by, the Oriental White Stork (Ciconia boyciana) used to be widely distributed across eastern Asia, more specifically in Russia, China, Japan and Korea. At present this species is only found in Russia and China, where an estimated 3000 individuals still survive (Simon and Derek, 2002). The main threats are hunting, insecticide application, fragmentation and loss of habitat and human disturbance, particularly so during the last three decades. This bird is now listed as endangered in the China Red Data Book (Zheng and Wang, 1998) and has entered the IUCN Red List of Threatened Species (IUCN, 2011).

    The Oriental White Stork is a migratory bird, breeding in northeastern Asia, wintering in southern areas near the Yangtze River in China (Wang and Yang, 1995). Recently, some Oriental White Storks were reported to breed in their wintering and stopover sites, such as Wuchang and Boyang lakes, Dafeng, Gaoyou and the Yellow River Delta (Xue et al., 2010), where some storks have remained for several generations. However, storks breeding in southeastern China show a low breeding rate, due to different weather conditions, extensive human activities and limited food resources (Hou et al., 2007; Yang et al., 2007).

    As indicated, the protection of this species is an urgent issue. Up to now ex-situ conservation has been the main conservation measure. In the 1980s, captive breeding of the Oriental White Stork was carried out in the Shanghai Zoo for the first time, where the number of captive storks has increased from 36, spread out over ten zoos in 1985, to 552 birds in 88 zoological gardens in 2004 (Ogawa, 2001; Ma et al., 2006), while its wild populations have declined in numbers (IUCN, 2011). It is anticipated that captive, reared storks will be released into the wild eventually (Collar et al., 2001).

    However, rearing birds over long periods in captivity may have changed their behavior. Understanding animal behavior is fundamental for ex-situ conservation (Jiang et al., 2001). Most captive animals are raised in limited spaces with an abundant food supply but strong human interference, which might affect their behavior (Curio, 1996; Armstrong et al., 1999). Previous research on captive storks has focused on breeding ecology, activity budgets, disease prevention and management (Coulter et al., 1989; Catherine, 1990; Murata and Asakawa, 1999; Li et al., 2002). Activity budgets are affected by many limiting factors, such as population density (Caraco, 1979; Yasue, 2005), size of territory (Hixon et al., 1983), human disturbance (Thomas et al., 2003), food richness (Hussell and Quinney, 1987; Litzow and Piatt, 2003), sex, age and weather (Verbeek, 1972; Caraco, 1979). In addition to these factors, space is another key factor, which could affect social structures (Jiang, 2004).

    The aim of our study was to investigate the behaviors of C. boyciana in a semi-natural enclosure in southeastern China over a period of a year, in order to understand the behavior in captivity, to compare these behaviors with those of storks in the wild at their southeastern breeding ground and to provide baseline information for conservation measures of this endangered species.

    This study was conducted from 15 October 2005 to 8 September 2006 in a semi-natural enclosure at the Hefei Wildlife Park in Hefei City, Anhui, China (31°50′ 3″N, 117°10′2″E). The study area is located between a temperate and a subtropical climate zone. The mean annual temperature is 15.7℃, ranging from −8.5℃ to 37.5℃ with a mean annual precipitation of about 969.5 mm and mean annual evaporation of 1265 mm. Atmospheric humidity ranged from 74%–78% and the frostfree period lasted 150 days in Hefei City. Meteorological data were collected from the China Meteorological Data Sharing Service System (http://cdc.cma.gov.cn/).

    The bottom of the enclosure is a circle with a diameter of 80 m, the top is completely covered by a nylon net supported by 12 small around pillars, 25 m in length and one big central pillar of 45 m. The enclosure for the storks covers an area of 0.5 ha. A pool is available in the middle of the enclosure. Three artificial nests were built on rocks, of which two nests were used in 2006. The dimensions of the first breeding nest are 2 m × 1.8 m and that of the second 1.8 m × 1.5 m. The smallest nest (1 m long and 1 m wide) was not selected by the storks for breeding. The main plants inside the enclosure were Cinnamonum camphora, Sapium sebiferum, Pinus thunbergii, Robinia pseudoacacia and a Phyllostachys sp. The following birds were kept together in the same enclosure: the Black Stork (Ciconia nigra), Red-crowned Crane (Grus japonensis), Swan Goose (Anser cygnoides), Ruddy Shelduck (Tadorna ferruginea), Black Swan (Cygnus atratus), Bean Goose (Anser fabalis) and Grey Heron (Ardea cinerea).

    Three male and three female Oriental White Storks were observed once a week from 15 October 2005 to 8 September 2006. Four birds established two mating pairs, while the two yearlings did not breed. These birds were assigned to three groups: a pair that produced three broods (Group 1); a pair that produced one brood (Group 2); a male and a female that were yearlings and did not breed (Group 3). We classified the behaviors of the storks into six modes: moving, breeding, defensive, foraging, preening and resting (Liu et al., 2001; Tian et al., 2005). Moving and foraging behaviors represent maintenance behaviors that are necessary for survival, while breeding, defensive and preening behaviors represent elastic behaviors not essential for survival (Jiang et al., 2001). Behavioral data were collected by means of instantaneous and scan sampling methods with 8 × 40 binoculars. All three groups were observed from 8:00–17:00 at five minute intervals once a week (Altmann, 1974). A total of 469 hours were spent on observations providing an average set of 5628 behavioral data for each stork and a total of 33768 observations for all storks. Each individual was identified by a different color ring on one leg. Age and sex of the individuals was gleaned from zoo records. The breeding season lasted from February to July.

    Figure  1.  (a) Habitat of Oriental White Storks in the Hefei Wildlife Park with two nest sites on artificial rock; (b) A brooding Oriental White Stork and another one guarding the nest.

    Temperature was monitored by a mercury thermometer (Shuangyu B-2, Ningbo, China) at thirty minute intervals from 8:00 to 17:00 for the duration of our field work (Yang and Yang, 1996). All temperature readings were averaged as one mean for each month for the duration of the observations. Daylight time was obtained from the National Time Service Center (NTSC) at the Chinese Academy of Sciences (http://www.time.ac.cn/serve/sunriseset/). Wind velocity was classified by the Beaufort Wind Force Scale (0–12 grades: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Beaufort_scale). We defined grade 0–1 as level 1, grade 2–3 as level 2, … and grade 10–12 as level 6 for a correlation analysis. We counted the number of the tourists in the zoo every ten minutes from 08:00 to 17:00, serving as an index of human disturbance. Monthly means were calculated for daylight time, wind velocity and the number of tourists in order to draw relationship graphs of behavior rhythm and the candidate factors at monthly intervals.

    Activity budgets were based on an index for each behavioral mode. It was calculated by dividing the frequency of the behavior mode by the total number of behavioral frequencies and expressed as percentages. Temperature, wind velocity, daylight and number of tourists were selected as candidate variables in order to investigate the relationship between activity budgets and predictor factors. However, this may not provide valid results due to redundant variables when the predictor variables are highly correlated (Wold et al., 1984). Collinearity analysis was carried out to detect correlations among the candidate variables. Our results showed that the value of the variance inflation factor (VIF) for the four candidate variables (temperature, wind velocity, daylight and number of tourists) were 4.345, 3.093, 2.903 and 2.064 respectively. Because each of the VIF values was less than 10, collinearity was not significant among the different candidate variables (Senter, 2008).

    Continuous variables were tested for normality (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test) and the results are presented as means ± standard errors. When the data met the assumption of normality, one-way ANOVAs were used to test time budgets among different groups and Pearson correlation coefficients were obtained to test the relationship between candidate variables and behavior frequency. When the data did not meet the assumptions of normality, a Kruskal-Wallis H and Spearman correlation were used for these analyses. All the statistical analyses were performed by Software SPSS 16.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

    The amount of time spent on maintenance behaviors (foraging and moving) accounted for 14.13% of total behavior time in Group 1, for 13.27% in Group 2 and for 13.49% in Group 3. The proportion of elastic behaviors, i.e., defensive, breeding and preening in Group 1 accounted for 33.34% for 37.60% in Group 2 and for 13.61% in Group 3 of total behavior time. These results suggest that the amount of time spent on foraging and moving, the so-called maintenance behaviors, was similar among the three groups, but the time spent on resting, defensive, breeding and preening behaviors was significantly different between the groups (Table 1). The proportion of time spent on all six types of behaviors did not show any significant sex-related differences (Table 2). The time spent on moving, resting, breeding and preening behaviors was significantly different between the breeding and non-breeding seasons, while the time spent on foraging and defensive behaviors was not (Table 3). Furthermore, differences in all six behavior modes were not explained by gender, so we could conclude that male and female storks show similar behavior budgets (Table 2). Defensive behavior in both males and females in driving out invading storks from the territory, as well as attendance at incubation, was observed in this experiment. Therefore our results, presented in Table 2, indicate that Oriental White Storks in the aviary were monogamous.

    Table  1.  Activity budget of different Oriental White Stork groups in the semi-natural enclosure in Hefei Wildlife Park
    Behavior Activity budget (%) Statistics test
    Group 1 (n=58) Group 2 (n=58) Group 3 (n=50) Chi-square or F values p
    Foraging 6.33 ± 4.25 6.25 ± 4.16 5.36 ± 4.50 2.85* p > 0.05
    Moving 7.80 ± 4.56 7.02 ± 3.52 8.13 ± 4.65 1.33* p > 0.05
    Resting 52.52 ± 17.67 49.13 ± 23.14 72.91 ± 8.35 51.07* p < 0.01
    Territorial 6.59 ± 3.15 6.90 ± 5.28 2.83 ± 2.27 40.79* p < 0.01
    Breeding 12.70 ± 19.73 17.24 ± 22.42 0.15 ± 0.47 39.53* p < 0.01
    Preening 14.05 ± 5.46 13.46 ± 8.03 10.63 ± 4.76 4.87 p < 0.01
    n: The number of observation days; * indicates the values from one-way ANOVA, indicates Chi-square values from a Kruskal-Wallis H test. Group 1: paired storks with three broods); Group 2: paired storks with one brood); Group 3: no paired storks.
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV
    Table  2.  Activity budget of Oriental White Stork of different sexes in a semi-natural enclosure in Hefei Wlidlife Park
    Behavior Activity budget (%) Statistics test
    Males ♀ (n = 94) Females (n = 90) Z or t values p
    Foraging 5.83 ± 4.11 6.26 ± 4.49 −0.74* p > 0.05
    Moving 7.81 ± 4.25 7.48 ± 4.33 −0.59* p > 0.05
    Resting 57.93 ± 18.98 56.01 ± 21.58 −0.25* p > 0.05
    Territorial 5.67 ± 3.78 5.65 ± 4.51 −0.48* p > 0.05
    Breeding 9.84 ± 17.29 11.64 ± 20.73 −0.40* p > 0.05
    Preening 12.92 ± 5.91 12.96 ± 6.83 −0.47 p > 0.05
    * indicates the values from independent sample T test, and indicates Chi-square values from Mann-Whitney U test (Similar comments apply to Table 3)
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV
    Table  3.  Activity budgets of Oriental White Stork between breeding and non-breeding seasons in a semi-natural enclosure in Hefei Wildlife Park
    Behavior Activity budget (%) Statistics test
    Breeding season (n = 138) Non-breeding season (n = 46) Z or t values p
    Foraging 6.95 ± 4.79 5.74 ± 4.09 −1.66* p > 0.05
    Moving 5.65 ± 3.51 8.31 ± 4.32 −3.80* p < 0.01
    Resting 41.19 ± 16.34 62.26 ± 18.68 −6.60* p < 0.01
    Territorial 6.18 ± 3.47 5.49 ± 4.34 −1.63* p > 0.05
    Breeding 25.07 ± 21.06 5.94 ± 15.66 −8.34* p < 0.01
    Preening 14.96 ± 7.02 12.26 ± 6.00 −2.34 p < 0.05
     | Show Table
    DownLoad: CSV

    The ambient temperature showed substantial annual variation throughout the year ranging from 1.8℃ to 47.6℃, with minimum temperatures in January and February and maximum temperatures in July and August (Fig. 2a). There was significant positive correlation between temperature and preening (Spearman rank correlation: r = 0.23, n = 178, p < 0.01), as well as with resting behavior (r = 0.18, n = 178, p < 0.05) and a negative correlation between temperature and defensive behavior (Pearson correlation: r = −0.23, n = 178, p < 0.01), but no correlation with foraging (r = 0.14, n = 178, p > 0.05) and breeding behavior (r = 0.07, n = 178, p > 0.05)(Fig. 2a).

    Figure  2.  Relationship analysis of factors affecting the behavior of Oriental White Storks from October 2005 to September 2006 in Hefei Wildlife Park. The factors affecting behavior consist of the following: (a) mean monthly temperature; (b) mean daily daylight; (c) wind velocity; (d) mean number of tourist recorded every five minutes.

    Variation in the amount of daylight ranged from 665 to 907 min per day during the study period, decreasing from June to December and increasing from December to June (Fig. 2b). There was a significant positive correlation between daylight time and moving (r = 0.22, n = 178, p < 0.01) and between daylight time and defensive behavior (r = 0.25, n = 178, p < 0.01), but a significant negative correlation with foraging (r= −0.16, n = 178, p < 0.05) and breeding (r = −0.19, n = 178, p < 0.05), while no correlation with preening (Spearman rank correlation: r = −0.008, p > 0.05) and resting behavior (r = 0.1, n = 178, p > 0.05) was observed (Fig. 2b).

    Wind velocity during the study period did not show substantial variation within the first four levels, with a minimum velocity in January and a maximum velocity in August (Fig. 2c). There was no correlation between wind velocity and defensive behavior (r = 0.09, n = 178, p > 0.05), moving (r = 0.01, n = 178, p > 0.05), resting (r = −0.13, n = 178, p > 0.05), foraging (r = −0.04, n = 178, p > 0.05) or breeding behavior (r = 0.07, n = 178, p > 0.05), but significant positive correlation of wind velocity with preening behavior (r = 0.22, n = 178, p < 0.01) (Fig. 2c).

    A maximum number of tourists was recorded in April and the least in November with the number of tourists, counted every five minutes, ranging from 4 to 11 (Fig. 2d). There was a significant positive correlation between the number of tourists and breeding (r = 0.18, n = 178, p < 0.05), foraging (r = 0.22, n = 178, p < 0.01), preening (r = 0.23, n = 178, p < 0.01) and defensive behavior (r = 0.19, n = 178, p < 0.01), a significant negative correlation with resting behavior (r = −0.28, n = 178, p < 0.01) and no correlation with moving behavior (r = −0.11, n = 178, p > 0.05).

    Time budgets of animal behaviors are the result of evolutional adaptation to the environment, where individuals with the best activity budget could benefit from natural selection (Verbeek, 1972; Yang and Yang, 1996). Previous research showed that behavioral structure is positively correlated with space. A study on the Coral-billed Ground-cuckoos (Carpococcyx renauldi) suggests greater frequency of defensive calls are recorded with less space (Hughes, 1997). Our results also showed that Groups 1 and 2 spent 6.59% and 6.90% of their time on defensive behavior, respectively (Table 1), with both times much higher than the 2.05% spent by the Oriental White Storks in a cage (dimensions of the cage: 4 m long × 3 m wide and 7 m in height (Liu et al., 2001). The preening behavior in Group 1 was 14.05%, 13.46% in Group 2 and 10.63% in Group 3 of the total behavior time which was higher than similar behavior of storks in a cage (8.9%, reported by Tian et al. 2005). This means that elastic behavior increases with increased space. However, the time spent on maintenance behavior (foraging and moving) for Groups 1, 2 and 3 were 14.13%, 13.27% and 13.49% respectively (Table 1), i.e., quantitatively similar to that of captive storks (13.5%, reported by Liu et al., 2001). But, the wild storks at Wuchang Lake spent 61.8% of their time foraging and moving (Yang et al., 2007). The foraging time spent by the storks in our semi-natural enclosure during this study, i.e., 6.33% for Group 1, 6.25% for Group 2 and 5.36% for Group 3 (Table 1) and in a cage (6.4%, reported by Tian et al. 2005) were much lower than those of wild storks in Anqing. Previous research on the White Stork (Ciconia ciconia), a sister species of the Oriental White Stork, showed that the digestive efficiency of males and females differed in terms of time spent on foraging (Kwiecinski and Tryjanowski, 2009). The differences in foraging time were not recorded for the Oriental White Stork (F94, 2= −0.74, p > 0.05; Table 2). Food availability was probably the key factor that prevented such a difference since food was provided by humans (Whittingham et al., 2000). Therefore, there was no relationship between maintenance behavior and enclosure size.

    When the temperature is high in the winter, the birds would spend more time in foraging for energy supplements. But when the temperature is very low, the increasing time spent on foraging may lead to greater energy loss than energy intake on the part of these birds. It is the reason why birds make trade-offs between feeding activity and energy loss (Cherel et al., 1988). Previous research on White Storks found that these birds would reduce time spent on foraging and moving in order to achieve the least energy loss in winter (Mata et al., 2001). In contrast, when the temperature was high, birds would increase the amount of time spent on foraging for energy intake. Our results indicated that defensive, preening and resting behavior are positive correlated with temperature (Fig. 2a); hence we speculated that temperature is an important factor for regulating the behavior of the Oriental White Stork. When the temperature is outside the usual threshold (too high or too low), this stork decreases the time spent on elastic behaviors (breeding, defensive and preening) and increase time on maintenance (moving and foraging) for a balance in energy consumption.

    Human activities affect animal behavior by inducing negative social or physiological consequences (Thomas et al., 2003; Yasue, 2005). Hou et al. (2007) found that the Oriental White Stork in the wild tolerates human presence at a distance of 87.6 m for warning and 50.7 m to escape, but when the intensity of human disturbance increased or the distance between animals and people decreased, birds would give up foraging in order to escape. The Oriental White Storks in the semi-natural enclosure were disturbed by human activities. We found that breeding, foraging, preening, resting and defensive behavior was affected by human activities (Fig. 2d). One way of minimizing the interference of visitors is to increase the distance between visitors and storks and to teach visitors about the negative impact they might have on these birds in zoos and aviaries.

    The size of the enclosure, human disturbance and environmental factors (temperature and daylight) affect the activity budget of the Oriental White Stork, hence more space and less human disturbance should be emphasized for this species in zoos.

    We would like to extend our sincere thanks to Hao JIANG, director of Hefei Wildlife Park, for his help on the behavior samples collection. We also thank Prof. Shengxian ZHONG for his help in correcting our English-language manuscripts. This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No. 3087317, 30470257) and the Anhui Academic and Technical Leaders Fund.

  • Barzen JA, Engels M, Burnham J, Harris J, Wu GF. Potential impacts of a water control structure on the abundance and distribution of wintering waterbirds at Poyang Lake. J Jpn Statist Soc. 2009;38:293-309.
    Beauchamp G. Group-size effects on vigilance: a search for mechanisms. Behav Process. 2003;63:111-21.
    Cao L, Barter MA, Lei G. New population estimates for Anatidae spending the non-breeding season in eastern China: implications for flyway population estimates. Biol Conserv. 2008;141:2301-9.
    Cheng YQ, Cao L, Barter M, Xu W, Zhang Y, Zhao M. Wintering waterbird Survey at the Anhui Shengjin Lake National Nature Reserve, China 2008/2009. Hefei: University of Science and Technology of China Press; 2009.
    Fang J, Wang Z, Zhao S, Li Y, Tang Z, Yu D, Ni L, Liu H, Xie P, Da L, Li Z, Zheng C. Biodiversity changes in the lakes of the Central Yangtze. Front Ecol Environ. 2006;4:369-77.
    Fischer JB, Griffin CR. Feeding behavior and food habits of wintering Harlequin Ducks at Shemya Island, Alaska. Wilson Bull. 2000;112:318-25.
    Fox AD, Hearn RD, Cao L, Cong PH, Wang X, Zhang Y, Dou ST, Shao XF, Barter M, Rees E. Preliminary observations of diurnal feeding patterns of swan geese Anser cygnoides using two different habitats at Shengjin Lake, Anhui Province, China. Wildfowl. 2008;58:20-30.
    Fox AD, Cao L, Zhang Y, Barter M, Zhao MJ, Meng FJ, et al. Declines in the tuber-feeding waterbird guild at Shengjin Lake National Nature Reserve, China-a barometer of submerged macrophyte collapse. Aquat Conser Mar Freshw Ecosyst. 2011;21:82-91.
    Gillespie GE, Kronlund AR. A manual for intertidal clam surveys. Canadian technical report of fisheries and aquatic science 2270, Fisheris and Oceans Canada, Pacific Biological Station; 1999.
    Godvik IMR, Loe LE, Vik JO, Vr V, Langvatn R, Mysterud A. Temporal scales, trade-offs, and functional responses in red deer habitat selection. Ecology. 2009;90:699-710.
    Guillemette M. The effect of time and digestion constraints in Common Eiders while feeding and diving over blue mussel beds. Funct Ecol. 1998;12:123-31.
    Gyimesi A, Stillman RA, Nolet BA. Cryptic interference competition in swans foraging on cryptic prey. Anim Behav. 2010;80:791-7.
    Hill DA, Ellis N. Survival and age related changes in the foraging behaviour and time budget of Tufted Ducklings Aythya fuligula. Ibis. 1984;126:544-50.
    Inger R, Bearhop S, Robinson JA, Ruxton G. Prey choice affects the trade-off balance between predation and starvation in an avian herbivore. Anim Behav. 2006;71:1335-41.
    Jiang HX, Xu WB, Qian FW, Chu GZ. Impact of habitat evolvement and human disturbance on wintering water birds in Shengjin Lake of Anhui Province, China. Chin J Appl Ecol. 2007;18(8):1832-6.
    Krebs JR. Optimal foraging: Decision rules for predators. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB, editors. Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Oxford: Blackwell; 1978. p. 23-63.
    Krebs JR, Stephens DW, Sutherland WJ. Perspectives in optimal foraging. In: Bush AH, Clark Jr CA, editors. Perspectives in ornithology: essays presented for the centennial of the American Ornithologists' Union. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1983. p. 165-221.
    Kuwae T, Miyoshi E, Sassa S, Watabe Y. Foraging mode shift in varying environmental conditions by dunlin Calidris alpina. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2010;406:281-9.
    Lewis TL, Esler D, Boyd WS. Effects of predation by sea ducks on clam abundance in soft-bottom intertidal habitats. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2007;329:131-44.
    Lewis TL, Esler D, Boyd WS. Foraging behavior of surf scoters (Melanitta perspicillata) and white-winged scoters (M. Fusca) in relation to clam density: inferring food availability and habitat quality. Auk. 2008;125:149-57.
    Li Z, Wang Z, Ge C. Time budgets of wintering Red-Crowned Cranes: effects of habitat, age and family size. Wetlands. 2013;33:227-32.
    Li C, Zhou L, Xu L, Zhao N, Beauchamp G. Vigilance and activity time-budget adjustments of wintering hooded cranes, Grus monacha, in human-dominated foraging habitats. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(3):e0118928.
    Littell RC, Pendergast J, Natarajan R. Modeling covariance structure in the analysis of repeated measures data. Stat Med. 2000;19:1793-819.
    Lovvorn JR. Distributional responses of Canvasback ducks to weather and habitat change. J Appl Ecol. 1989;26:113-30.
    May R, Mclean A. Theoretical ecology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2010.
    Meng RX. The aquatic vegetation of Shengjin Lake. J Anhui Univ (Nat Sci Sect). 1979;3:71-80 (in Chinese).
    Nolet BA, Klaassen M. Retrodicting patch use by foraging swans in a heterogeneous environment using a set of functional responses. Oikos. 2009;118:431-43.
    Pyke GH. Optimal foraging theory: a critical review. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1984;15:523-75.
    Pyke GH, Pulliam HR, Charnov EL. Optimal foraging: a selective review of theory and tests. Quart Rev Biol. 1977;52:137-54.
    Richman SE, Lovvorn JR. Effects of clam species dominance on nutrient and energy acquisition by Spectacled Eiders in the Bering Sea. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2003;261:283-97.
    Stephens DW, Krebs JR. Foraging theory. New Jersey: Princeton University Press; 1986.
    Systad GH, Bustnes JO, Erikstad KE. Behavioral responses to decreasing day length in wintering sea ducks. Auk. 2000;117:33-40.
    Tome MW. Optimal foraging: food patch depletion by Ruddy Ducks. Oecologia. 1988;76:27-36.
    Yang L, Zhou L, Song Y. The effects of food abundance and disturbance on foraging flock patterns of the wintering Hooded Crane (Grus monacha). Avian Res. 2015;6:1-8.
    Yasué M. Environmental factors and spatial scale influence shorebirds' responses to human disturbance. Biol Conserv. 2006;128:47-54.
    Zhang JX, Lu JJ. Feeding ecology of two wintering geese species at Poyang Lake, China. J Freshw Ecol. 1999;14:439-45.
    Zheng M, Zhou L, Zhao N, Xu W. Effects of variation in food resources on foraging habitat use by wintering Hooded cranes (Grus monacha). Avian Res. 2015;6:1-10.
    Zhou B, Zhou L, Chen J, Xu W, Cheng Y. Assemblage dynamics and territorial behaviour of hooded crane in winter at Shengjin Lake. Chin J Wildl. 2009;30:133-6 (in Chinese)
  • Related Articles

  • Cited by

    Periodical cited type(8)

    1. Sue-Jeong Jin, Hae-Ni Kim, Jun-Seo Go, et al. Comparative analysis of female bubbling calls: Within- and between-species variation among the four species of Cuculus cuckoos. Avian Research, 2025. DOI:10.1016/j.avrs.2025.100240
    2. Csaba Moskát, Márk E. Hauber. On the sparrowhawk-like calls of female common cuckoos: testing for heterospecific vocal mimicry in a conspecific functional context. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 2023, 77(10) DOI:10.1007/s00265-023-03388-6
    3. Swetlana G. Meshcheryagina, Alexey Opaev. Previously unknown behavior in parasitic cuckoo females: male-like vocalization during migratory activity. Avian Research, 2021, 12(1) DOI:10.1186/s40657-021-00246-9
    4. Jennifer E. York. The Evolution of Predator Resemblance in Avian Brood Parasites. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 2021, 9 DOI:10.3389/fevo.2021.725842
    5. Jiaojiao Wang, Qihong Li, Canchao Yang. Coevolution of acoustical communication between obligate avian brood parasites and their hosts. Avian Research, 2020, 11(1) DOI:10.1186/s40657-020-00229-2
    6. Iliana Medina, Rebecca M Kilner, Naomi E Langmore, et al. From micro- to macroevolution: brood parasitism as a driver of phenotypic diversity in birds. Current Zoology, 2020, 66(5): 515. DOI:10.1093/cz/zoaa033
    7. Zhuqing Deng, Huw Lloyd, Canwei Xia, et al. Components of variation in female common cuckoo calls. Behavioural Processes, 2019, 158: 106. DOI:10.1016/j.beproc.2018.10.007
    8. Canwei Xia, Zhuqing Deng, Huw Lloyd, et al. The function of three main call types in common cuckoo. Ethology, 2019, 125(9): 652. DOI:10.1111/eth.12918

    Other cited types(0)

Catalog

    Figures(1)  /  Tables(6)

    Article Metrics

    Article views (504) PDF downloads (17) Cited by(8)

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return